全文获取类型
收费全文 | 38486篇 |
免费 | 1467篇 |
国内免费 | 20篇 |
专业分类
39973篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 398篇 |
2019年 | 532篇 |
2018年 | 712篇 |
2017年 | 789篇 |
2016年 | 745篇 |
2015年 | 570篇 |
2014年 | 653篇 |
2013年 | 3272篇 |
2012年 | 1125篇 |
2011年 | 1233篇 |
2010年 | 805篇 |
2009年 | 822篇 |
2008年 | 1156篇 |
2007年 | 1080篇 |
2006年 | 1001篇 |
2005年 | 889篇 |
2004年 | 830篇 |
2003年 | 856篇 |
2002年 | 806篇 |
2001年 | 1309篇 |
2000年 | 1268篇 |
1999年 | 954篇 |
1998年 | 485篇 |
1997年 | 378篇 |
1996年 | 375篇 |
1995年 | 384篇 |
1994年 | 376篇 |
1993年 | 358篇 |
1992年 | 784篇 |
1991年 | 760篇 |
1990年 | 714篇 |
1989年 | 670篇 |
1988年 | 641篇 |
1987年 | 627篇 |
1986年 | 598篇 |
1985年 | 677篇 |
1984年 | 540篇 |
1983年 | 434篇 |
1981年 | 353篇 |
1979年 | 504篇 |
1978年 | 400篇 |
1977年 | 381篇 |
1976年 | 365篇 |
1975年 | 469篇 |
1974年 | 469篇 |
1973年 | 518篇 |
1972年 | 419篇 |
1968年 | 406篇 |
1967年 | 363篇 |
1966年 | 418篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
Return of fear: underlearning and overlearning 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
92.
93.
94.
95.
On second-order family therapy 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
S Golann 《Family process》1988,27(1):51-71
If family therapists remain aware of the indivisible and recursive nature of their interactions with families, and if they use this awareness to form a collaborative rather than a hierarchical therapeutic system, and at the same time minimize their attempts to change persons or family structures in strategic or predetermined ways, then they may be said to be practicing a "second-order" family therapy. This article analyzes the development of the second-order position on therapist power and influence, concluding that it is inconsistent and possibly disingenuous. 相似文献
96.
97.
S Baron-Cohen 《Cognition》1988,29(1):83-84
98.
Spatial coding in the processing of anaphor by good and poor readers: Evidence from eye movement analyses 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Wayne S. Murray Alan Kennedy 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》1988,40(4):693-718
An experiment that examined the way in which young readers deployed eye movements while reading sentences and while answering questions containing either a pronominal or noun anaphor is reported. To evaluate the possible causal role played by differences in inspection strategies between readers of above- and below-average reading skill, a third“age control” group of younger children was also tested. This group was matched on absolute reading ability with the less skilled group of older children, and on relative reading ability (i.e. reading quotient) with the more skilled group. Differences in inspection strategy were apparent between the groups of good and poor readers. Good readers launched more selective reinspections, whereas the poorer readers were more inclined to engage in“backtracking” and appeared to make less use of the displayed text. In every case there was a marked similarity in the behaviour of the good readers and the“age controls”. These results suggest that the ability to code the spatial location of words in a sentence, and, where necessary, to use this information to launch accurately targetted selective reinspections of previously read text, plays a crucial role in the development of skilled reading performance. 相似文献
99.
The distorted room illusion (DRI) and the attendant argument for perceptual ambiguity is critically analyzed from a Gibsonian/ecological point of view. The notions of multiple specification, conflicting information, and perceptual skill are invoked in showing how the ecological approach can accommodate illusion effects that may remain under mobile binocular viewing conditions. Static optic arrays are shown not to be ambiguous. So-called equivalent configurations are found to be analytic artifacts, appearing when the problem of information is treated in geometrical terms without regard for constraints due to physical and ecological regularities. The relative importance of motion-based and motion-independent information is discussed. 相似文献
100.
Three experiments tested the hypothesis that people can execute focused searches of semantic cases when answering questions about complex facts, such as “the teacher watered the peas, the corn, and the lettuce with the hose.” In Experiment 1, answer time varied mainly with the number of concepts in the relevant or focused case, supporting the hypothesis. Experiment 2 indicated that the irrelevant case undergoes some processing. Experiment 3 confirmed that search in Experiments 1 and 2 focused on semantic cases rather than on taxonomic categories. It is proposed that focused memory search has a high probability of retrieving the relevant case and a low probability of retrieving the irrelevant case. In the latter event, the irrelevant concepts receive full processing. 相似文献