全文获取类型
收费全文 | 7179篇 |
免费 | 282篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
7463篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 56篇 |
2020年 | 110篇 |
2019年 | 125篇 |
2018年 | 155篇 |
2017年 | 180篇 |
2016年 | 172篇 |
2015年 | 154篇 |
2014年 | 178篇 |
2013年 | 872篇 |
2012年 | 286篇 |
2011年 | 294篇 |
2010年 | 177篇 |
2009年 | 210篇 |
2008年 | 295篇 |
2007年 | 285篇 |
2006年 | 262篇 |
2005年 | 267篇 |
2004年 | 252篇 |
2003年 | 234篇 |
2002年 | 232篇 |
2001年 | 109篇 |
2000年 | 101篇 |
1999年 | 115篇 |
1998年 | 124篇 |
1997年 | 84篇 |
1996年 | 85篇 |
1995年 | 95篇 |
1994年 | 79篇 |
1993年 | 70篇 |
1992年 | 72篇 |
1991年 | 71篇 |
1990年 | 57篇 |
1989年 | 65篇 |
1988年 | 61篇 |
1987年 | 53篇 |
1986年 | 55篇 |
1985年 | 76篇 |
1984年 | 73篇 |
1983年 | 68篇 |
1982年 | 60篇 |
1981年 | 62篇 |
1980年 | 63篇 |
1979年 | 44篇 |
1978年 | 63篇 |
1977年 | 57篇 |
1976年 | 63篇 |
1975年 | 58篇 |
1974年 | 54篇 |
1973年 | 44篇 |
1972年 | 39篇 |
排序方式: 共有7463条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
92.
Karen Colapietro Seybold Paul R. Salomone 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1994,73(1):4-9
Is workaholism an addiction or a rewarding work style and life-style that may be harmful to one's family and co-workers? This central question directed the review of definitions and causes of workaholism. Thereafter, the authors discuss the role of “Type A” and obsessive-compulsive traits on the behavior of some workaholics, review the effects of workaholism on workaholics themselves and on others, and suggest counseling approaches that might be helpful to workaholics. 相似文献
93.
The Stability of Personality: Observations and Evaluations 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
94.
95.
96.
97.
Latent Structure of the Sources of Mathematics Self-Efficacy 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Robert W. Lent Frederick G. Lopez Steven D. Brown Paul A. Gore Jr. 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》1996,49(3):292-308
General social cognitive theory and its career-specific elaborations posit four primary sources through which self-efficacy beliefs are acquired and modified: personal performance accomplishments, vicarious learning, social persuasion, and physiological states and reactions. We present two studies exploring the dimensionality of these sources within the context of career-relevant mathematics activities. In Study 1, 295 college students completed measures of the source variables. Testing two- through five-factor models, we found strongest support for a four-factor latent structure of the efficacy sources. In Study 2, involving 481 high school students, a five-factor model fit the data well. We also found evidence of a higher order factor structure in both samples. Several directions for further research on the sources of efficacy information are considered, along with implications for career and academic interventions. 相似文献
98.
99.
Peter Suedfeld Brian de Vries Susan Bluck Alistair B. C. Wallbaum Paul W. Schmidt 《International journal of psychology》1996,31(5):177-190
Although there are measurable differences in integrative complexity among solutions that individuals generate in dealing with problems, it is uncertain to what extent people comprehend, recognize, and have preferences among different levels of complexity. Integrative complexity is a function of differentiation (the perception of several attributes within, or perspectives about, a topic) and integration (combining the differentiated characteristics in an interactive or synthesizing solution). The current paper reports two experiments dealing with how university students perceive, interpret, and choose among solutions differing in complexity. Experiment 1 showed that subjects accurately rated the complexity of described solutions differing along the continuum, but that their assessment of their own responses differed from the results of expert scoring. Their self-estimated complexity was highly correlated with their preferences, and preferred complexity was reliably higher than either expert- or self-assessed complexity of subject-generated solutions. Subjects were able to hypothesize quite accurately about environmental and endogenous factors likely to affect complexity. Experiment 2 found that in response to problem scenarios, solutions selected as being potentially most effective were consistently more complex than solutions that participants considered themselves most likely to use. The idea of complexity seems to be intuitively recognizable and understandable by untrained subjects: They can and do distinguish among problem solutions (self-generated or presented) that vary on that dimension, and are able to assess accurately the effects of relevant variables. Such subjects also share the bias shown by experts in favour of the superiority of more complex approaches. 相似文献
100.
Recent research in risk perception has examined the role of affect and worldviews as orienting dispositions that guide people's decisions about complex and risky topics such as nuclear energy. This study tests and supports the hypothesis that worldviews and affect-laden imagery are highly predictive of perceptions of risk from nuclear power and support for that technology. Furthermore, affect and worldviews each contribute independently to the prediction of nuclear support. We find also that a person's affective imagery associated with nuclear power is systematically related to their worldviews. We conclude that affect and worldviews appear to play similar roles as orienting mechanisms, helping people navigate in a complex, uncertain, and sometimes dangerous world. The implication of this view for the practice of risk communication is briefly discussed. 相似文献