全文获取类型
收费全文 | 53752篇 |
免费 | 977篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
54734篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 538篇 |
2019年 | 561篇 |
2018年 | 744篇 |
2017年 | 844篇 |
2016年 | 882篇 |
2015年 | 670篇 |
2014年 | 788篇 |
2013年 | 4183篇 |
2012年 | 1426篇 |
2011年 | 1510篇 |
2010年 | 960篇 |
2009年 | 973篇 |
2008年 | 1398篇 |
2007年 | 1315篇 |
2006年 | 1188篇 |
2005年 | 1142篇 |
2004年 | 1077篇 |
2003年 | 1033篇 |
2002年 | 1103篇 |
2001年 | 1446篇 |
2000年 | 1427篇 |
1999年 | 1081篇 |
1998年 | 600篇 |
1997年 | 507篇 |
1992年 | 944篇 |
1991年 | 888篇 |
1990年 | 909篇 |
1989年 | 892篇 |
1988年 | 834篇 |
1987年 | 799篇 |
1986年 | 852篇 |
1985年 | 934篇 |
1984年 | 768篇 |
1983年 | 713篇 |
1982年 | 539篇 |
1981年 | 582篇 |
1979年 | 800篇 |
1978年 | 625篇 |
1977年 | 556篇 |
1976年 | 532篇 |
1975年 | 775篇 |
1974年 | 844篇 |
1973年 | 831篇 |
1972年 | 745篇 |
1971年 | 684篇 |
1970年 | 633篇 |
1969年 | 710篇 |
1968年 | 825篇 |
1967年 | 791篇 |
1966年 | 760篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
201.
This essay provides guidelines for designing a doctoral program in behavior analysis. First, we propose a general accomplishment for all behavior analytic doctoral students: that they be able to solve problems concerning individual behavior within a range of environments. Second, in order to achieve this goal, we propose that students be trained in conceptual and experimental analysis of behavior, the application of behavioral principles and the administration of behavioral programs. This training should include class work, but it should emphasize the immersion of students in a variety of environments in which they are required to use behavior analytic strategies. Third, we provide an example of a hypothetical graduate program that involves the proposed training. Finally, an evaluation plan is suggested for determining whether a training program is in fact producing students who are generalized problem-solvers. At each step, we justify our point of view from a perspective that combines principles from behavior analysis and educational systems design. 相似文献
202.
An experiment with 42 human Ss used the transswitching procedure to examine tonic stimulus control of phasic and tonic conditioned vasomotor heart rate, and electrodermal reactions. The conditional stimulus (CSs) were photos of angry and friendly human faces, and the unconditional stimulus (US) was a human scream. In one tonic context (blue light), the CSs were paired with the US, in the other context (yellow light), the CSs were presented unpaired. Following acquisition, an extinction series was run with the US omitted during both tonic contexts. Phasic vasomotor and skin conductance reactions differed in the positive and negative tonic segments (stronger in positive). The skin conductance responses also differed during extinction, but the vasomotor responses did not. Tonic differences (following onset of the tonic stimuli) in unelicited skin conductance response frequency, finger pulse volume, and heart rate were also found, although these developed more slowly than the phasic differences. The finger pulse volume tonic difference was greater in extinction than the skin conductance response frequency. There was no effect of the angry-friendly facial expressions, either directly or in interaction with the transswitching effects. The results were interpreted to mean that the transswitching phenomenon is not limited to one another autonomic effector, but is more generalized across the ANS (sympathetic branch). The absence of influence of the facial expressions indicates the relative weakness of the “preparedness” hypothesis in comparison with more influential contextual factors. 相似文献
203.
Paul C. Quinn B. R. Wooten Evette J. Ludman 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1985,37(3):198-204
Sternheim and Boynton’s (1966) continuous judgmental color-naming technique was used to assess the elemental nature of achromatic colors. Results from three subjects indicate that the names “black” and “white” are necessary and sufficient to describe achromatic test lights associated with the name “gray.” On the basis of the criteria that establishes a color as elemental, black and white were confirmed as elemental, whereas gray was not. These findings support the assumption of various opponent processing models that there are only two elemental achromatic sensations. In addition, the results support Hering’s (1920) view that black and white differ from the paired-chromatic opponent colors in that they are not mutually exclusive. 相似文献
204.
205.
H K Gediman 《Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association》1985,33(4):911-935
This paper deals with imposturous tendencies as ubiquitous and heterogeneous. They may enter into neurotic conflict and compromise, and also reflect an ego function disturbance involving multiple, shifting identities and subsequent problems in the subjective sense of reality of the self and objects. Imposture in a person undergoing analysis is, however, not only a function of individual character and psychopathology; it is also a function of certain inevitable requirements of the analytic situation which constitute a "pull" for its emergence. Vulnerable individuals will respond to this pull in revealing ways. Three case summaries illustrate the spectrum of imposturous tendencies. 相似文献
206.
207.
This study concerns the manner in which the traumatic events suffered by the parent in the Nazi holocaust are communicated to the second generation born after the Second World War. Additionally we investigated the effects of differences in wartime experiences on subsequent communication behaviors. Communication behaviors in families whose parents had been prisoners in concentration camps were compared with those of families who had displayed active resistance (i.e., were partisans) during the Second World War. The data indicate greater legitimacy and openness in discussing holocaust-related issues in the homes of ex-partisans than in the homes of ex-prisoners in concentration camps. Also, sons and daughters of the former group have better knowledge of the holocaust and hold more favorable attitudes than do sons and daughters in the second group. The implications of these findings are discussed. 相似文献
208.
H L Piersma 《Adolescence》1985,20(80):975-979
Some of the common conflicts that occur between therapists and direct-care staff in residential, adolescent treatment facilities are highlighted. These conflicts often parallel those occurring between an adolescent's parents. Common issues include the amount of time spent with the adolescent patient, questions of authority, and communication difficulties. These conflicts can have positive consequences, if each party recognizes that the other brings a needed perspective to adolescent treatment. 相似文献
209.
210.