首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   29204篇
  免费   1025篇
  国内免费   13篇
  2020年   310篇
  2019年   333篇
  2018年   466篇
  2017年   536篇
  2016年   525篇
  2015年   380篇
  2014年   472篇
  2013年   2488篇
  2012年   862篇
  2011年   931篇
  2010年   569篇
  2009年   587篇
  2008年   848篇
  2007年   828篇
  2006年   741篇
  2005年   704篇
  2004年   679篇
  2003年   631篇
  2002年   678篇
  2001年   825篇
  2000年   844篇
  1999年   661篇
  1998年   376篇
  1997年   295篇
  1996年   279篇
  1992年   517篇
  1991年   473篇
  1990年   496篇
  1989年   475篇
  1988年   452篇
  1987年   443篇
  1986年   458篇
  1985年   491篇
  1984年   403篇
  1983年   365篇
  1982年   295篇
  1979年   428篇
  1978年   339篇
  1977年   307篇
  1976年   279篇
  1975年   396篇
  1974年   424篇
  1973年   393篇
  1972年   366篇
  1971年   315篇
  1970年   282篇
  1969年   341篇
  1968年   382篇
  1967年   349篇
  1966年   360篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
Tibbetts  Paul 《Synthese》2004,141(2):247-276
Synthese - The concept of voluntary motor control(VMC) frequently appears in the neuroscientific literature, specifically in the context of cortically-mediated, intentional motor actions. For...  相似文献   
32.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
33.
34.
35.
Even though it can be shown that verbal knowledge of results (KR) is redundant with sensory feedback for learning certain motor skills, such findings do not eliminate the possibility that when KR is available it influences underlying learning processes. In order to examine the function of KR more closely, two experiments were designed in which the subjects received conflicting information about their own sensory feedback and the KR presented by the experimenter. In Experiment 1, two erroneous-KR groups, a correct-KR group, and a no-KR group performed 150 practice trials on a simple anticipation timing task and then performed three no-KR retention tests of 30 trials each following intervals of 10 minutes, 1 week, and 1 month. The results supported previous findings that providing correct KR is redundant in anticipation tasks. However, learning was influenced by KR as subjects performed according to the erroneous KR information, thereby ignoring their sensory feedback even after a 1-month interval. In Experiment 2, subjects practised a more complex striking response for the anticipation task for 75 trials and then performed no-KR retention trials either immediately, or 1 day or 1 week later. One of the groups received erroneous KR after 50 practice trials with correct KR. The results confirmed and extended those from Experiment 1, as erroneous KR, even after initial practice with correct KR, influenced retention performance. These results indicate that although KR provides information that is not needed to learn anticipation timing skills, this augmented verbal information is a dominant source of information that influences underlying cognitive processes involved in learning motor skills.  相似文献   
36.
37.
38.
39.
40.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号