全文获取类型
收费全文 | 23658篇 |
免费 | 471篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
24130篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 234篇 |
2019年 | 348篇 |
2018年 | 410篇 |
2017年 | 399篇 |
2016年 | 431篇 |
2015年 | 334篇 |
2014年 | 437篇 |
2013年 | 2136篇 |
2012年 | 715篇 |
2011年 | 765篇 |
2010年 | 472篇 |
2009年 | 512篇 |
2008年 | 709篇 |
2007年 | 721篇 |
2006年 | 676篇 |
2005年 | 606篇 |
2004年 | 616篇 |
2003年 | 558篇 |
2002年 | 560篇 |
2001年 | 555篇 |
2000年 | 607篇 |
1999年 | 456篇 |
1998年 | 324篇 |
1997年 | 235篇 |
1996年 | 248篇 |
1995年 | 259篇 |
1994年 | 227篇 |
1992年 | 346篇 |
1991年 | 355篇 |
1990年 | 352篇 |
1989年 | 338篇 |
1988年 | 327篇 |
1987年 | 287篇 |
1986年 | 301篇 |
1985年 | 331篇 |
1984年 | 261篇 |
1983年 | 276篇 |
1982年 | 218篇 |
1980年 | 216篇 |
1979年 | 292篇 |
1978年 | 282篇 |
1975年 | 271篇 |
1974年 | 298篇 |
1973年 | 309篇 |
1972年 | 258篇 |
1971年 | 242篇 |
1969年 | 220篇 |
1968年 | 276篇 |
1967年 | 266篇 |
1966年 | 242篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
Direction- and velocity-specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field in the middle temporal visual area (MT) 总被引:7,自引:0,他引:7
The true receptive field of more than 90% of neurons in the middle temporal visual area (MT) extends well beyond the classical receptive field (crf), as mapped with conventional bar or spot stimuli, and includes a surrounding region that is 50 to 100 times the area of the crf. These extensive surrounds are demonstrated by simultaneously stimulating the crf and the surround with moving stimuli. The surrounds commonly have directional and velocity-selective influences that are antagonistic to the response from the crf. The crfs of MT neurons are organized in a topographic representation of the visual field. Thus MT neurons are embedded in an orderly visuotopic array, but are capable of integrating local stimulus conditions within a global context. The extensive surrounds of MT neurons may be involved in figure-ground discrimination, preattentive vision, perceptual constancies, and depth perception through motion cues. 相似文献
72.
The Bem Sex-Role Inventory and the Communication-Conflict Instrument were administered to 182 husbands and wives. Results suggest that gender-role classification is a more significant discriminator of communication behavior than is biological sex. Androgynous spouses, more than feminine though not significantly more so than masculine spouses, reported themselves to be the most disposed to handle conflict constructively. On the subscales of the conflict instrument, androgynous persons had significantly more positive feelings scores than either masculine or feminine persons, plus higher task energy and desire-for-control scores than feminine persons. Similarly, masculine persons reported a significantly greater predisposition to manage conflict constructively than did feminine persons, having higher task energy and desire-for-control scores. Undifferentiated persons reported the lowest scores for conflict management. 相似文献
73.
Hardyck, Tzeng, and Wang (1978, Brain and Language, 5, 56–71) hypothesized that ample repetition of a small number of stimuli is required in order to obtain VHF differences in tachistoscopic tasks. Four experiments, with varied levels of repetition, were conducted to test this hypothesis. Three experiments utilized the general task of object-picture naming and one utilized a word-naming task. Naming latencies constituted the dependent measure. The results demonstrate that for the object-naming paradigm repetition is required for RVF superiority to emerge. Repetition was found to be unnecessary for RVF superiority in the word-naming paradigm, with repetition actually reducing RVF superiority. Experiment I suggested the possibility that RVF superiority developed for the second half of the trials as a function of practice or hemispheric activation, regardless of repetition level. Subsequent experiments, better designed to assess this possibility, clearly refuted it. It was concluded that the effect of repetition depends on the processing requirements of the task. We propose that, for tasks which can be processed efficiently by one hemisphere, the effect of repetition will be to reduce VHF asymmetries; but tasks requiring substantial processing by both hemispheres will show shifts to RVF superiority as a function of repetition. 相似文献
74.
75.
Paul C. Quinn B. R. Wooten Evette J. Ludman 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1985,37(3):198-204
Sternheim and Boynton’s (1966) continuous judgmental color-naming technique was used to assess the elemental nature of achromatic colors. Results from three subjects indicate that the names “black” and “white” are necessary and sufficient to describe achromatic test lights associated with the name “gray.” On the basis of the criteria that establishes a color as elemental, black and white were confirmed as elemental, whereas gray was not. These findings support the assumption of various opponent processing models that there are only two elemental achromatic sensations. In addition, the results support Hering’s (1920) view that black and white differ from the paired-chromatic opponent colors in that they are not mutually exclusive. 相似文献
76.
A common criticism of iterative least squares estimates of communality is that method of initial estimation may influence stabilized values. As little systematic research on this topic has been performed, the criticism appears to be based on cumulated experience with empirical data sets. In the present paper, two studies are reported in which four types of initial estimate (unities, squared multiple correlations, highestr, and zeroes) and four levels of convergence criterion were employed using four widely available computer packages (BMDP, SAS, SPSS, and SOUPAC). The results suggest that initial estimates have no effect on stabilized communality estimates when a stringent criterion for convergence is used, whereas initial estimates appear to affect stabilized values employing rather gross convergence criteria. There were no differences among the four computer packages for matrices without Heywood cases. 相似文献
77.
Fear of death and the concept of extended self 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
78.
Effects of presentation complexity on rapid-sequential reading 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
79.
Cross-language evidence for three factors in speech perception 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
80.
This project studied the effects of weather variables and size of the population on minor and major violence rates within six prisons in the Kingston, Ontario, Canada area from January, 1980 through December, 1983. While weather variables have been studied in relation to mood changes within the general population, this study was the first to analyze the impact of weather variables on rates of violence within the prison setting using forward inclusion multiple regression. Analysis showed that population size consistently appeared positively correlated with assaults on other inmates among the male medium-security prisoners during the summer months. In the summer months, temperature tended to be positively related to some minor kinds of offenses, but in the winter months the relationship was negative. Humidity, rain, sunshine hours, snow, and snow on the ground were not consistently related to incidences of minor and major violence. Indices of geomagnetic disturbances had statistically significant inverse relationships with attempted suicide/self-inflicted injury rates among the male prisoners during the summer months. Over 12 months, wind was generally negatively correlated with incidences of major and minor violence among the male inmates. For 6-mo. periods, wind was generally positively correlated with violence rates during the winter months and negatively correlated during the summer months among the male inmates. The findings relating weather variables to violence rates in the women's prison were consistently in opposition to those found for male prisoners during the 4-yr. period. 相似文献