全文获取类型
收费全文 | 327篇 |
免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
332篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 6篇 |
2018年 | 10篇 |
2017年 | 7篇 |
2015年 | 6篇 |
2014年 | 8篇 |
2013年 | 24篇 |
2012年 | 16篇 |
2011年 | 13篇 |
2010年 | 4篇 |
2008年 | 16篇 |
2007年 | 11篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 12篇 |
2004年 | 8篇 |
2003年 | 9篇 |
2002年 | 7篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 8篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 8篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1967年 | 4篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1964年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有332条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
131.
Annie A. Garner Janice C. Marceaux Sylvie Mrug Cryshelle Patterson Bart Hodgens 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2010,38(8):1097-1107
The present study examined Sluggish Cognitive Tempo (SCT) in relation to ADHD symptoms, clinical diagnosis, and multiple aspects
of adjustment in a clinical sample. Parent and teacher reports were gathered for 322 children and adolescents evaluated for
behavioral, emotional, and/or learning problems at a university clinic. Confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) supported the presence
of three separate, but correlated factors (SCT, inattention, and hyperactivity/impulsivity) in both parent and teacher ratings.
As expected, SCT symptoms were greatest in youth with ADHD Inattentive type, but were also found in non-ADHD clinical groups.
SCT symptoms were related to inattention, internalizing, and social problems across both parent and teacher informants; for
parent reports, SCT was also related to more externalizing problems. Findings support the statistical validity of the SCT
construct, but its clinical utility is still unclear. 相似文献
132.
A traditional control group yoked to a group that self-controls their reception of feedback receives feedback in the same relative and absolute manner. This traditional control group typically does not learn the task as well as the self-control group. Although the groups are matched for the amount of feedback they receive, the information is provided on trials in which the individual may not request feedback if he or she were provided the opportunity. Similarly, individuals may not receive feedback on trials for which it would be a beneficial learning experience. Subsequently, the mismatch between the provision of feedback and the potential learning opportunity leads to a decrement in retention. The present study was designed to examine motor learning for a yoked group with the same absolute amount of feedback, but who could self-control when they received feedback. Increased mental processing of error detection and correction was expected for the participants in the yoked self-control group because of their choice to employ a limited resource in the form of a decreasing amount of feedback opportunities. Participants in the yoked with self-control group committed fewer errors than the self-control group in retention and the traditional yoked group in both the retention and time transfer blocks. The results suggest that the yoked with self-control group was able to produce efficient learning effects and can be a viable control group for further motor learning studies. 相似文献
133.
Contrary to Vaesen's argument that humans are unique with respect to nine cognitive capacities essential for tool use, we suggest that although such cognitive processes contribute to variation in tool use, it does not follow that these capacities are necessary for tool use, nor that tool use shaped cognition per se, given the available data in cognitive neuroscience and behavioral biology. 相似文献
134.
Semantic memory encompasses knowledge of objects, facts and words. A number of brain regions are probably involved, but the left infero-lateral temporal lobe appears to play a key role. The separability of semantic memory from episodic (or autobiographical) memory is a focus of current debate. Impaired semantic memory is a common feature of Alzheimer's disease but is invariably overshadowed by a profound deficit in episodic memory. In semantic dementia, a rarer disorder associated with focal temporal-lobe atrophy, there is selective loss of semantic memory, characterized by preservation of superordinate knowledge of words, and objects, but loss of finer-grained information. This pattern can be interpreted as a degradation of features from a distributed network of semantic representations. Following Herpes simplex encephalitis, patients sometimes show disproportionate loss of knowledge for natural kinds (e.g. animals) with relative preservation of knowledge about artefacts, this may reflect differential damage to neural systems critical for perceptual as opposed to functional features, perceptual properties being more salient in knowledge about natural kinds. 相似文献
135.
A strategy for the study of social interaction in the family is reviewed that places particular emphasis on those variables that have proven useful or hold promise for the identification and understanding of factors that affect human social development. First, the heavy reliance on observational data and other main features of the approach are described. Second, an attempt is made to summarize some of the more significant findings from this type of research that have added significantly to our understanding of social development within the family. Third, the major research methods used in this approach are described, as well as the research that needs to be done in order to improve the methodology. Finally, a number of major gaps in our substantive knowledge about social interaction within the family are indicated. 相似文献
136.
137.
K Patterson J L Bradshaw 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1975,1(3):246-252
Studies employing schematic faces of three features explored the role of the left hemisphere as an analytic processor in difficult discriminations and the role of the right hemisphere in easy gestalt matches. In a discrimination task, the successively presented members of a stimulus pair differed in all three features. When both stimuli went to the same visual field there was a Visual Field X Judgments interaction; judgments same were processed faster in the left visual field. When a test stimulus differing on all three features from a previously memorized target face was matched to the latter, both judgments same and different were made faster for stimuli in the left visual field. However, when the test stimulus and the memory items differed on only one feature the right field proved superior for both judgments same and different. 相似文献
138.
Habituation in a protozoan Vorticella convallaria 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
D J Patterson 《Behaviour》1973,45(3):304-311
139.
The energy crisis of the winter of 1973-74 led to severe shortages of fuel oil for home heating and a government request for voluntary conservation by the oil consumer. This experiment tested two methods of facilitating fuel-oil conservation. Home fuel-oil consumers were randomly assigned to one of three experimental groups: feedback of information on rate of oil use, feedback plus commendation for reduced consumption, or a no-treatment control. The consumption rate for the feedback plus commendation group was significantly lower than that of either the informational feedback group or the control group. The informational feedback group did not differ from the control group. The results suggest that feedback alone may not result in oil conservation, but that feedback combined with commendation can produce socially significant savings. 相似文献
140.