首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2680篇
  免费   405篇
  国内免费   5篇
  3090篇
  2023年   18篇
  2022年   17篇
  2021年   44篇
  2020年   55篇
  2019年   64篇
  2018年   147篇
  2017年   154篇
  2016年   178篇
  2015年   117篇
  2014年   87篇
  2013年   315篇
  2012年   152篇
  2011年   188篇
  2010年   99篇
  2009年   147篇
  2008年   116篇
  2007年   134篇
  2006年   95篇
  2005年   81篇
  2004年   84篇
  2003年   79篇
  2002年   87篇
  2001年   45篇
  2000年   41篇
  1999年   27篇
  1998年   32篇
  1997年   27篇
  1996年   20篇
  1995年   35篇
  1994年   24篇
  1993年   19篇
  1992年   19篇
  1991年   18篇
  1990年   21篇
  1989年   16篇
  1988年   17篇
  1987年   21篇
  1986年   17篇
  1985年   23篇
  1984年   14篇
  1983年   13篇
  1982年   19篇
  1981年   19篇
  1980年   15篇
  1979年   13篇
  1978年   15篇
  1977年   15篇
  1976年   10篇
  1975年   15篇
  1974年   15篇
排序方式: 共有3090条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
42.
43.
44.
45.
46.
组织公平文献综述及未来的研究方向   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
林帼儿  陈子光  钟建安 《心理科学》2006,29(4):1016-1018
文章的主要目的在于回顾组织公平自其产生至今的主要文献,并介绍Colquitt(2001)的组织公平四因素结构(即分配公平、程序公平、人际公平和信息公平)[1]。文章还讨论了组织公平的工具性和非工具性模型。最后,文章介绍了组织公平今后的几个研究方向。  相似文献   
47.
The main determinants of lexical access in speech are considered to be a word's age of acquisition (AoA) and its frequency of occurrence in a speaker's experience. It is unclear whether and how these variables interact, although they are commonly observed to be correlated, for the few studies that address the issue have reported inconsistent findings. An influential view of AoA in lexical processing (Ellis and Lambon Ralph, 2000) predicts stronger frequency effects for items acquired later in life than for those acquired at an early age. Five experiments were designed to investigate the possible interaction of AoA and frequency effects in speech. We found that the interaction between word frequency and AoA was not robust and that, contrary to expectation, the effect of word frequency was greater for words acquired earlier in life than for those acquired later. The implications of our findings are discussed.  相似文献   
48.
A common response of animals to physical restraint is tonic immobility. The authors observed the behavior of garter snakes, Thamnophis elegans, in the field to determine the frequency of immobility as a response to handling. Most snakes fled after release, but the remainder remained immobile, sometimes on their backs (supination), for up to 10 min. Immobility was seen most often in pregnant snakes, which also were more likely than other snakes not to have moved before capture. Failure to move, either before or after capture, might be a consequence of the limited locomotory ability of gravid snakes. However, the lack of observations of interactions between snakes and their natural predators impedes researchers' understanding of the antipredator value, if any, of tonic immobility.  相似文献   
49.
Socio-cultural perspectives explain inconsistency in Chinese child-rearing research when imported methods and concepts are applied. We sought to elucidate child-rearing belief constructs relevant to Chinese contexts. Exploratory factor analysis identified items representing child-rearing concepts both imported and indigenous to Chinese culture and forming four dimensions: Training, Shame, Authoritative, and Autonomy. Data from parents of preschool-aged children in Hong Kong ( N  = 228) and Taiwan ( N  = 213) were subject to confirmatory factor and scale internal consistency analyses. Results support the conceptual and psychometric coherence of each subscale. Discussion of findings focuses on the benefits of culturally responsive methodology and the potential utility of the scales in child-rearing research with Chinese and other Confucian cultural populations.  相似文献   
50.
ABSTRACT— Although people generally appear optimistic about the future, they shift from optimism under certain circumstances. Drawing from a recent review of the literature, we describe how both optimism and shifts from optimism serve the common goal of preparedness, which includes a readiness to deal with setbacks and a readiness to take advantage of opportunities. Shifts from optimism occur in response to available information and to the possibility that things may not turn out as hoped. People tend to shift from optimism when feedback is anticipated in the near future, when the outcome is important, when negative outcomes are easily imagined, and when the outcomes are uncontrollable. In addition, people with low self-esteem shift from optimism more readily than do people with high self-esteem. Finally, both optimism and shifts from optimism have unique benefits in terms of preparedness.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号