全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2322篇 |
免费 | 385篇 |
专业分类
2707篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 14篇 |
2022年 | 15篇 |
2021年 | 35篇 |
2020年 | 47篇 |
2019年 | 52篇 |
2018年 | 137篇 |
2017年 | 145篇 |
2016年 | 165篇 |
2015年 | 111篇 |
2014年 | 74篇 |
2013年 | 287篇 |
2012年 | 132篇 |
2011年 | 169篇 |
2010年 | 86篇 |
2009年 | 140篇 |
2008年 | 101篇 |
2007年 | 119篇 |
2006年 | 88篇 |
2005年 | 71篇 |
2004年 | 65篇 |
2003年 | 71篇 |
2002年 | 75篇 |
2001年 | 33篇 |
2000年 | 32篇 |
1999年 | 26篇 |
1998年 | 24篇 |
1997年 | 19篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1995年 | 31篇 |
1994年 | 20篇 |
1993年 | 17篇 |
1992年 | 16篇 |
1991年 | 15篇 |
1990年 | 17篇 |
1989年 | 13篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1987年 | 15篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 18篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 17篇 |
1981年 | 17篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 12篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 11篇 |
1976年 | 10篇 |
1975年 | 11篇 |
1974年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有2707条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
Alice van de Walle de Ghelcke Xanthi Skoura Martin Gareth Edwards Patrick Quercia Charalambos Papaxanthis 《Journal of Neuropsychology》2021,15(2):215-234
Developmental dyslexia (DD), a severe and frequent disorder of reading acquisition, is characterized by a diversity of cognitive and motor deficits whose interactions still remain under debate. Although deficits in the automatization of sensorimotor control have been highlighted, internal action representation allowing prediction has never before been investigated. In this study, we considered action representation of 18 adolescents with pure DD and 18 age-matched typical readers. Participants actually and mentally performed a visually guided pointing task involving strong spatiotemporal constraints (speed/accuracy trade-off paradigm). While actual and mental movement times of typical readers were isochronous and both conformed to Fitts’ law, the movement times of dyslexics differed between conditions, and only the actual movement times conformed to Fitts’ law. Furthermore, the quality of motor imagery correlated with word reading abilities. This suggests that the process of action representation is impaired in pure DD and supports the sensorimotor perspective of DD. Theoretical implications are discussed. 相似文献
72.
Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a relatively highly prevalent psychiatric disorder that is associated with very high personal and socioeconomic costs. This paper provides a state-of-the-art review of the relationship between complex trauma and key features of BPD, with a focus on problems with self-coherence and self-continuity. We first review evidence for the high prevalence of complex trauma in BPD patients. This is followed by a discussion of emerging knowledge concerning the biobehavioral mechanisms involved in problems related to self and identity in BPD. We emphasize three biobehavioral systems that are affected by complex trauma and are centrally implicated in identify diffusion in BPD: the attachment system, mentalizing or social cognition, and the capacity for epistemic trust—that is, an openness to the reception of social communication that is personally relevant and of generalizable significance. We formulate a new approach to personality and severe personality disorders, and to problems with self and identity in these disorders, rooted in a social-communicative understanding of the foundations of selfhood. We also discuss how extant evidence-based treatments address the above-mentioned biobehavioral systems involved in identity diffusion in BPD and related disorders, and the supporting evidence. We close the paper with recommendations for future research. 相似文献
73.
74.
Recognizing images: The role of motivational significance,complexity, social content,age, and gender
Patrick Gomez Armin von Gunten Brigitta Danuser 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2020,61(2):183-194
Memory for affective events plays an important role in determining people’s behavior and well-being. Its determinants are far from being completely understood. We investigated how recognition memory for affective pictures depends on pictures’ motivational significance (valence and arousal), complexity (figure-ground compositions vs. scenes), and social content (pictures with people vs. without people) and on observers’ age and gender. Younger, middle-aged, and older adults viewed 84 pictures depicting real-life situations. After a break, the participants viewed 72 pictures, half of which had been viewed previously and half of which were novel, and were asked to endorse whether each picture was novel or had been presented previously. Hits, false alarms, and overall performance (discrimination accuracy) were our dependent variables. The main findings were that, across participants, recognition memory was better for unpleasant than pleasant pictures and for pictures depicting people than pictures without people. Low-arousal pictures were more accurately recognized than high-arousal pictures, and this effect was significantly larger among middle-aged and older adults than younger adults. Recognition memory worsened across adulthood, and this decline was steeper among men than women. Middle-aged and older women outperformed their male counterparts. This study suggests that how well we are able to successfully discriminate previously seen pictorial stimuli from novel stimuli depends on several pictures’ properties related to their motivational significance and content, and on observer’s age and gender. 相似文献
75.
Ludovic Ferrand Stphanie Ducrot Pierre Chausse Norbert Maïonchi‐Pino Richard J. OConnor Benjamin A. Parris Patrick Perret Kevin J. Riggs Maria Augustinova 《Developmental science》2020,23(2)
Only one previous developmental study of Stroop task performance (Schiller, 1966) has controlled for differences in processing speed that exist both within and between age groups. Therefore, the question of whether the early developmental change in the magnitude of Stroop interference actually persists after controlling for processing speed needs further investigation; work that is further motivated by the possibility that any remaining differences would be caused by process(es) other than processing speed. Analysis of data from two experiments revealed that, even after controlling for processing speed using z‐transformed reaction times, early developmental change persists such that the magnitude of overall Stroop interference is larger in 3rd‐ and 5th graders as compared to 1st graders. This pattern indicates that the magnitude of overall Stroop interference peaks after 2 or 3 years of reading practice (Schadler & Thissen, 1981). Furthermore, this peak is shown to be due to distinct components of Stroop interference (resulting from specific conflicts) progressively falling into place. Experiment 2 revealed that the change in the magnitude of Stroop interference specifically results from joint contributions of task, semantic and response conflicts in 3rd‐ and 5th graders as compared to a sole contribution of task conflict in 1st graders. The specific developmental trajectories of different conflicts presented in the present work provide unique evidence for multiple loci of Stroop interference in the processing stream (respectively task, semantic and response conflict) as opposed to a single (i.e. response) locus predicted by historically – favored response competition accounts. 相似文献
76.
77.
78.
79.
80.
Patrick Grim 《Synthese》1993,94(3):409-428
Predicates are term-to-sentence devices, and operators are sentence-to-sentence devices. What Kaplan and Montague's Paradox of the Knower demonstrates is that necessity and other modalities cannot be treated as predicates, consistent with arithmetic; they must be treated as operators instead. Such is the current wisdom.A number of previous pieces have challenged such a view by showing that a predicative treatment of modalities neednot raise the Paradox of the Knower. This paper attempts to challenge the current wisdom in another way as well: to show that mere appeal to modal operators in the sense of sentence-to-sentence devices is insufficient toescape the Paradox of the Knower. A family of systems is outlined in which closed formulae can encode other formulae and in which the diagonal lemma and Paradox of the Knower are thereby demonstrable for operators in this sense.I am deeply indebted to Robert F. Barnes and Evan W. Conyers, without whom these ideas might not have germinated and certainly would not have grown. Many of the results offered here evolved in the course of mutual discussion and correspondence. I am also grateful to an anonymous reviewer forSynthese for many very helpful suggestions.The current paper contains the technical results promised in Footnote 25 of Grim (1988) and Footnote 26, Chapter 3, of Grim (1991). 相似文献