全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2633篇 |
免费 | 317篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
2951篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 18篇 |
2022年 | 17篇 |
2021年 | 45篇 |
2020年 | 54篇 |
2019年 | 60篇 |
2018年 | 143篇 |
2017年 | 151篇 |
2016年 | 177篇 |
2015年 | 116篇 |
2014年 | 84篇 |
2013年 | 300篇 |
2012年 | 145篇 |
2011年 | 183篇 |
2010年 | 96篇 |
2009年 | 144篇 |
2008年 | 113篇 |
2007年 | 129篇 |
2006年 | 95篇 |
2005年 | 76篇 |
2004年 | 81篇 |
2003年 | 76篇 |
2002年 | 79篇 |
2001年 | 40篇 |
2000年 | 42篇 |
1999年 | 26篇 |
1998年 | 29篇 |
1997年 | 23篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1995年 | 33篇 |
1994年 | 24篇 |
1993年 | 19篇 |
1992年 | 18篇 |
1991年 | 16篇 |
1990年 | 19篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 15篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 13篇 |
1985年 | 21篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 17篇 |
1981年 | 18篇 |
1980年 | 12篇 |
1979年 | 12篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 12篇 |
1976年 | 10篇 |
1975年 | 12篇 |
1974年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有2951条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
201.
Psychotherapy can no longer be understood as a morally neutral or value-free practice. Many have argued that psychology is implicitly and inevitably laden with moral assumptions and has a significant impact on the moral and ethical fabric of its clients and surrounding society (Browning 1987; Doherty 1995; Dueck and Reimer 2003; Kelly and Strupp 1992; Rieff 1987, 2006). In this work, the authors highlight the profound identity crises within the field of psychology around the place of morality/ethics in its theories and practices. Chronicling some of the shifts between modern and postmodern theory will allow for a meaningful engagement with this issue. The postmodern shifts in the field have left clinicians ideologically hampered in their ability to attend to the moral dimensions of their clients’ lives. In conclusion, the authors posit a virtue ethics and consider avenues of holistically and meaningfully infusing clinical practice with moral sensibilities. 相似文献
202.
203.
Ferrand, Segui, and Grainger (1996) found robust syllable priming effects in picture naming latencies: Pictures primed with their initial syllable (e.g., ba for baleine [whale]) were processed faster than pictures primed with a string of letters shorter or longer than their initial syllable (e.g., bal for baleine). However, in several studies, these priming effects have not been replicated in word naming or in picture naming either in Dutch or in English (Schiller, 1998, 1999, 2000). The present study was aimed at replicating syllable priming effects in picture naming in French using a masked priming paradigm. The study employed a larger number of participants and items than were used in the Ferrand et al. (1996) study. The syllable priming effect in picture naming latencies was not replicated. Subsampling procedures were then used to examine the stability of the Ferrand et al. (1996) pattern of results in picture naming in greater detail. The syllabic priming effect in picture naming turned out to be an extremely rare event. 相似文献
204.
Claes L Vandereycken W Luyten P Soenens B Pieters G Vertommen H 《Journal of personality disorders》2006,20(4):401-416
A three factor model of personality pathology was investigated in a clinical sample of 335 female eating disordered patients. Cluster analysis of the Big Five NEO-FFI scales (Costa & McCrae, 1992) yielded three distinct personality profiles, which were consistent with previous studies: (1) a resilient/high functioning cluster with no clinical elevations on the NEO-FFI scales; (2) an undercontrolled/emotionally dysregulated cluster with elevated scores on the Neuroticism scale and low scores on Conscientiousness and Agreeableness; (3) an overcontrolled/constricted cluster showing high scores on Neuroticism and Conscientiousness and low scores on Openness to Experience. Comparing the three personality prototypes with respect to Axis I and Axis II disorders,resilients reported systematically less clinical and personality problems than both undercontrollers and overcontrollers. Compared to the latter, undercontrollers showed more impulsive personality features and behaviors. Finally, cluster membership was not clearly associated with eating disorder subtypes, suggesting that there is considerable variance in personality features and/or pathology within the various eating disorder categories. 相似文献
205.
Subjects typically experience the temporal interval immediately following a saccade as longer than a comparable control interval. One explanation of this effect is that the brain antedates the perceptual onset of a saccade target to around the time of saccade initiation. This could explain the apparent continuity of visual perception across eye movements. This antedating account was tested in three experiments in which subjects made saccades of differing extents and then judged either the duration or the temporal order of key events. Postsaccadic stimuli underwent subjective temporal lengthening and had early perceived onsets. A temporally advanced awareness of saccade completion was also found, independently of antedating effects. These results provide convergent evidence supporting antedating and differentiating it from other temporal biases. 相似文献
206.
Gregory PT Gregory LA 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》2006,120(3):262-268
A common response of animals to physical restraint is tonic immobility. The authors observed the behavior of garter snakes, Thamnophis elegans, in the field to determine the frequency of immobility as a response to handling. Most snakes fled after release, but the remainder remained immobile, sometimes on their backs (supination), for up to 10 min. Immobility was seen most often in pregnant snakes, which also were more likely than other snakes not to have moved before capture. Failure to move, either before or after capture, might be a consequence of the limited locomotory ability of gravid snakes. However, the lack of observations of interactions between snakes and their natural predators impedes researchers' understanding of the antipredator value, if any, of tonic immobility. 相似文献
207.
208.
209.
210.
This study explored the relationships between perceived family processes (parental bonding and parental discipline styles) and adolescent emotional and behavioural adjustment among a sample of youth. Respondents were 92 (53 female, 39 male) high school students, aged 13–17 years. Significant sex differences were noted regarding levels of self-reported delinquency and parental induction style. After controlling for sex, perceptions of low care and love withdrawal were significantly related to delinquency, while low care, overprotectiveness and love withdrawal were found to be related to poor well being. Structural equation modelling was used to further assess the relationships between perceptions of family life and the outcome variables. It was concluded that adolescent perceptions of family processes form a coherent and integrated network with implications for behavioural and emotional adjustment. 相似文献