全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2607篇 |
免费 | 311篇 |
专业分类
2918篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 17篇 |
2022年 | 16篇 |
2021年 | 39篇 |
2020年 | 51篇 |
2019年 | 55篇 |
2018年 | 138篇 |
2017年 | 148篇 |
2016年 | 169篇 |
2015年 | 114篇 |
2014年 | 77篇 |
2013年 | 306篇 |
2012年 | 139篇 |
2011年 | 174篇 |
2010年 | 92篇 |
2009年 | 147篇 |
2008年 | 109篇 |
2007年 | 124篇 |
2006年 | 96篇 |
2005年 | 73篇 |
2004年 | 67篇 |
2003年 | 76篇 |
2002年 | 78篇 |
2001年 | 37篇 |
2000年 | 35篇 |
1999年 | 27篇 |
1998年 | 25篇 |
1997年 | 21篇 |
1996年 | 19篇 |
1995年 | 33篇 |
1994年 | 24篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 22篇 |
1991年 | 20篇 |
1990年 | 17篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 16篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 14篇 |
1985年 | 21篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 16篇 |
1982年 | 18篇 |
1981年 | 19篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 15篇 |
1978年 | 19篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 12篇 |
1975年 | 16篇 |
1974年 | 16篇 |
排序方式: 共有2918条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
111.
Volume Contents
Contents Volume 相似文献112.
This paper reports some experimental results on the coordination of finger and vocal responses with passing through a target position in multjjoint arm movement. In Experiment 1, we found that the difference in the timing of finger and vocal responses cannot be attributed entirely to efferent or representational effects. Instead, it appears to reflect the extent to which information about the internal stimuli generated by the arm movement are available to the centers controlling these different responses. That is, it is a compatibility effect. In Experiment 2, the case in which a finger response is made on the same side of the body as the moving arm was compared with the case in which it is made with the contralateral hand, which remains static. The interaction effect observed suggests that the path-ways subserving coordinated responses are informationally encapsulated, so that information about arm movement is not shared between the neural centers controlling different coordinated responses. 相似文献
113.
114.
Fiona N. Newell Patrick Chiroro Tim Valentine 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》1999,52(2):509-534
Three experiments are reported in which the effects of viewpoint on the recognition of distinctive and typical faces were explored. Specifically, we investigated whether generalization across views would be better for distinctive faces than for typical faces. In Experiment 1 the time to match different views of the same typical faces and the same distinctive faces was dependent on the difference between the views shown. In contrast, the accuracy and latency of correct responses on trials in which two different faces were presented were independent of viewpoint if the faces were distinctive but were view-dependent if the faces were typical. In Experiment 2 we tested participants'recognition memory for unfamiliar faces that had been studied at a single three-quarter view. Participants were presented with all face views during test. Finally, in Experiment 3, participants were tested on their recognition of unfamiliar faces that had been studied at all views. In both Experiments 2 and 3 we found an effect of distinctiveness and viewpoint but no interaction between these factors. The results are discussed in terms of a model of face representation based on inter-item similarity in which the representations are view specific. 相似文献
115.
Patrick Stokes 《Continental Philosophy Review》2006,39(4):393-413
The ethics expressed in Kierkegaard’s Works of Love has been subject to persistent criticism for its perceived indifference to concrete persons and failure to attend to the other in their individual specificity. Recent defenses of Works of Love have focused in large part on the role of vision in the text, showing the supposed “blind” empty formalism of the emphasis on the category of “the neighbor” to serve a normative model of seeing the other correctly. However, when this problem is viewed in the broader context of Kierkegaard’s phenomenology of moral vision, two further, thus far unanswered, problems emerge: How can we see the other and the moral demand they represent at the same time, and how can we see the other and our own condition at the same time? This paper draws on other Kierkegaardian texts to show how Kierkegaard’s model of moral vision allows for the simultaneity in vision necessary to overcome these challenges. 相似文献
116.
To study the relationship of selected Holtzman Inkblot Technique (HIT) scores and anxiety, Ss were matched on the basis of A-Trait scores on the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). The HIT was administered to Ss under neutral and threat of shock conditions. State anxiety was monitored by means of the A-State scale of the STAI. None of the HIT variables reflected changes in A-State due to the experimental conditions. HIT anxiety correlated significantly with STAI A-Trait. Results are discussed in terms of Trait-State Anxiety theory and previous research. 相似文献
117.
This paper reports on an unusual study of survivors of rape and the effects on the victim, her significant male other person, and her parents. All survivors were victims of the same rapist and were interviewed from 21 months to ten years after the event. The study indicates that the long-term effects of rape may be more significant than previous researchers and practitioners thought was the case. Recommendations are given for more effective help for survivors and those close to them.This study was funded by the Ohio Department of Mental Health, Office of Program Evaluation and Research, Columbus, Ohio, 1985. 相似文献
118.
Patrick Maher 《Studia Logica》2007,86(2):331-341
How can formal methods be applied to philosophical problems that involve informal concepts of ordinary language? Carnap answered this question by describing a methodology that he called “explication." Strawson objected that explication changes the subject and does not address the original philosophical problem; this paper shows that Carnap’s response to that objection was inadequate and offers a better response. More recent criticisms of explication by Boniolo and Eagle are shown to rest on misunderstandings of the nature of explication. It is concluded that explication is an appropriate methodology for formal philosophy. 相似文献
119.
120.
Patrick H Knee CR Canevello A Lonsbary C 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2007,92(3):434-457
Self-determination theory posits 3 basic psychological needs: autonomy (feeling uncoerced in one's actions), competence (feeling capable), and relatedness (feeling connected to others). Optimal well-being results when these needs are satisfied, though this research has traditionally focused on individual well-being outcomes (e.g., E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan, 2000). Three studies examined the role of need fulfillment in relationship functioning and well-being. Study 1 found that fulfillment of each need individually predicted both individual and relationship well-being, with relatedness being the strongest unique predictor of relationship outcomes. Study 2 found that both partners' need fulfillment uniquely predicted one's own relationship functioning and well-being. Finally, in Study 3, the authors used a diary recording procedure and tested a model in which the association between need fulfillment and relationship quality was mediated by relationship motivation. Those who experienced greater need fulfillment enjoyed better postdisagreement relationship quality primarily because of their tendency to have more intrinsic or autonomous reasons for being in their relationship. 相似文献