全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2412篇 |
免费 | 23篇 |
专业分类
2435篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 27篇 |
2020年 | 28篇 |
2019年 | 35篇 |
2018年 | 63篇 |
2017年 | 53篇 |
2016年 | 63篇 |
2015年 | 51篇 |
2014年 | 53篇 |
2013年 | 266篇 |
2012年 | 88篇 |
2011年 | 99篇 |
2010年 | 65篇 |
2009年 | 71篇 |
2008年 | 92篇 |
2007年 | 99篇 |
2006年 | 77篇 |
2005年 | 89篇 |
2004年 | 87篇 |
2003年 | 85篇 |
2002年 | 75篇 |
2001年 | 39篇 |
2000年 | 40篇 |
1999年 | 37篇 |
1998年 | 38篇 |
1997年 | 31篇 |
1996年 | 34篇 |
1995年 | 30篇 |
1994年 | 31篇 |
1993年 | 38篇 |
1992年 | 27篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 33篇 |
1989年 | 36篇 |
1988年 | 32篇 |
1987年 | 24篇 |
1986年 | 21篇 |
1985年 | 21篇 |
1984年 | 28篇 |
1983年 | 28篇 |
1982年 | 30篇 |
1981年 | 25篇 |
1980年 | 22篇 |
1979年 | 13篇 |
1978年 | 27篇 |
1977年 | 11篇 |
1976年 | 19篇 |
1975年 | 17篇 |
1973年 | 18篇 |
1968年 | 12篇 |
1967年 | 13篇 |
排序方式: 共有2435条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
201.
Marcus B. Waller Patricia F. Waller 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1963,6(1):29-37
Two dogs were maintained on a multiple schedule having both a food reinforced and an avoidance component (Mult VI 1′ SΔ AvoidSS20 RS20 SΔ). The effects of superimposing an Estes-Skinner procedure for delivering unavoidable shocks on all components of the multiple schedule were observed. The buzzer-shock pairing of the Estes-Skinner procedure produced an increased rate of responding on the avoidance component of the schedule and also on the SΔ components. No persistent change in rate was observed on the food component during the pre-shock stimulus. Control performances on all components could be regained by either extinguishing or eliminating the buzzer-shock pairing. Extinction of the avoidance responding had little effect on the increased rates of responding produced by the Estes-Skinner procedure on the SΔ and avoidance extinction components and did not lead to a conditioned suppression of the food reinforced responding. Rate of responding during the pre-shock stimulus was observed to be relatively independent of changes in the maintaining schedules. Responding during the pre-shock stimulus could be conditioned and maintained after an extensive history of avoidance extinction. 相似文献
202.
Selma A. Lewis Jim Johnson Patricia Cohen Marc Garcia Carmen Noemi Velez 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1988,16(4):459-471
Twenty-six suicide attempters, 9 to 18 years of age, were compared with 725 nonattempters on measures of school achievement, educational goals, socioeconomic status, and depression. As has been found in studies using clinical samples, suicide attempters in our communitybased sample had significantly lower school achievement than nonattempters. The relationship between attempted suicide and low school achievement seemed to be explained by the effects of depression. Lower-SES youth tended to be more likely to attempt suicide than higher-SES youths. However, SES appeared to have little effect on the relationship between attempted suicide and school achievement. Results also suggest that children of mothers with low educational goals for them may be more at risk for attempted suicide than youths whose mothers have higher educational goals for their children.This research was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health, Grant No. MH36971. 相似文献
203.
Patricia Kitcher 《Pacific Philosophical Quarterly》1987,68(3-4):306-316
204.
205.
Dennis R. Moore Patricia Chamberlain Leona H. Mukai 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1979,7(3):345-355
This study compared the follow-up incidence of court-recorded nonstatus offenses for three groups of adolescent children. These children had been seen 2 to 9 years earlier for problems with aggression in the home (N = 21), for stealing problems (N = 25), or for normative comparisons (N = 14). The results showed that 77% of the children with stealing problems had court-recorded offenses. This was significantly higher than the aggressive children, whose rate did not differ from the normative sample. These findings suggested that young aggressive children were not at risk for adolescent court contact. Instead, it was the young child with identified stealing problems who was highly likely to become an official delinquent. It also appeared that parental reports of stealing events constituted a predictive measure of later criminal acts. 相似文献
206.
Patricia A. Reutter-Lorenz Louise Stanczak & Andrea C. Miller 《Psychological science》1999,10(6):494-500
Several neuroimaging studies have reported that older adults show weaker activations in some brain areas together with stronger activations in other areas, compared with younger adults performing the same task. This pattern may reflect neural recruitment that compensates for age-related neural declines. The recruitment hypothesis was tested in a visual laterality study that investigated age differences in the efficiency of bihemispheric processing. Letter-matching tasks of varying complexity were performed under two conditions: (a) matching letters projected to the same visual field (hemisphere) and (b) matching letters projected to opposite visual fields (hemispheres). As predicted by the recruitment hypothesis, older adults generally performed better in the bilateral than unilateral condition, whereas younger adults showed this pattern onlyfor the most complex task. We discuss the relation between these results and neuroimaging evidence for recruitment, and the relevance of the present bihemispheric advantage to other evidence for age-related changes in interhemispheric transfer. 相似文献
207.
High flanker recall in the correlated flanker task suggests a failure of selective attention. We used a dissociation approach to examine the role of attentional processing of the flankers’ identities in the flanker validity effect (FVE). In three experiments, flanker recall decreased as flanker duration decreased, but the FVE was not affected by flanker duration. Masking also produced a dissociation, in that masking decreased flanker recall but the FVE was not affected. Examination of on-line flanker identification suggested that flanker recall was an adequate measure of attentional processing of the flankers’ identities on most trials. The results suggest that attentional processing of the flankers’ identities is not related to the magnitude of the FVE and is not necessary for it to obtain. Because there is a failure of selective attention, the dissociations suggest that flanker recall and the FVE may reflect different attentional processes. 相似文献
208.
Frances Cogle Lawrence Peggy S. Draughn Grace E. Tasker Patricia H. Wozniak 《Sex roles》1987,17(9-10):489-502
A sample of 440 full-time employed couples from 11 states was examined to determine the differences between time spent on housework by husbands and wives in rural and urban areas. More time is spent in housework in rural families than in urban families because time spent in housework is greater for rural women than for urban women, while rural and urban husbands' times do not differ. Education of spouses, job status differentials, and age of the younger child do not significantly interact with rural-urban residence and sex of the spouse. However, family income does significantly influence the relationship between time spent on housework for rural and urban husbands and wives. 相似文献
209.
One of the most distinctive characteristics of humans is the capacity to learn from what other people tell them. Often new information is provided about an entity that is not present, requiring incorporation of that information into one's mental representation of the absent object. Here we present evidence regarding the emergence of this vital ability. Nineteen- and 22-month-old infants first learned a name for a toy and later were told that the toy had undergone a change in state (it had become wet) while out of view. The 22-month-olds (but not the 19-month-olds) subsequently identified the toy solely on the basis of the property that they were told about but had never seen. Thus, before the end of their 2nd year, infants can use verbal information to update their representation of an absent object. This developmental advance inaugurates a uniquely human and immensely powerful form of learning about the world. 相似文献
210.
Goodman GS Sayfan L Lee JS Sandhei M Walle-Olsen A Magnussen S Pezdek K Arredondo P 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2007,98(4):233-242
This study demonstrates that experience and development interact to influence the "cross-race effect." In a multination study (n=245), Caucasian children and adults of European ancestry living in the United States, Norway, or South Africa, as well as biracial (Caucasian-African American) children and adults living in the United States, were tested for recognition of Asian, African, and Caucasian faces. Regardless of national or biracial background, 8- to 10-year-olds, 12- to 14-year-olds, and adults recognized own-race faces more accurately than other-race faces, and did so to a similar extent, whereas 5- to 7-year-olds recognized all face types equally well. This same developmental pattern emerged for biracial children and adults. Thus, early meaningful exposure did not substantially alter the developmental trajectory. During young childhood, developmental influences on face processing operate on a system sufficiently plastic to preclude, under certain conditions, the cross-race effect. 相似文献