全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3008篇 |
免费 | 34篇 |
专业分类
3042篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 15篇 |
2021年 | 36篇 |
2020年 | 45篇 |
2019年 | 38篇 |
2018年 | 71篇 |
2017年 | 76篇 |
2016年 | 76篇 |
2015年 | 57篇 |
2014年 | 56篇 |
2013年 | 328篇 |
2012年 | 120篇 |
2011年 | 132篇 |
2010年 | 85篇 |
2009年 | 102篇 |
2008年 | 127篇 |
2007年 | 115篇 |
2006年 | 101篇 |
2005年 | 108篇 |
2004年 | 101篇 |
2003年 | 104篇 |
2002年 | 94篇 |
2001年 | 53篇 |
2000年 | 49篇 |
1999年 | 41篇 |
1998年 | 48篇 |
1997年 | 39篇 |
1996年 | 39篇 |
1995年 | 40篇 |
1994年 | 42篇 |
1993年 | 43篇 |
1992年 | 31篇 |
1991年 | 31篇 |
1990年 | 48篇 |
1989年 | 46篇 |
1988年 | 37篇 |
1987年 | 24篇 |
1986年 | 24篇 |
1985年 | 30篇 |
1984年 | 35篇 |
1983年 | 28篇 |
1982年 | 37篇 |
1981年 | 29篇 |
1980年 | 27篇 |
1979年 | 18篇 |
1978年 | 29篇 |
1977年 | 16篇 |
1976年 | 26篇 |
1975年 | 23篇 |
1973年 | 17篇 |
1967年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有3042条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
901.
Katalin Szanto Holly Prigerson Patricia Houck Lin Ehrenpreis Charles F. Reynolds 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》1997,27(2):194-207
This study examined whether symptoms of complicated grief at baseline predicted suicidal ideation during a depressive episode in elderly bereaved individuals. Over a 17-month period, serial ratings of suicidal ideation, hopelessness, and symptoms of depression, anxiety, and complicated grief were obtained from 130 elderly participants who had lost their spouses within the past 2 years. Groups of active and passive suicidal ideators, as well as nonideator controls, were compared via analysis of variance (ANOVA) with respect to levels of complicated grief, depression, and anxiety. Elderly bereaved with both active and passive suicidal ideation were found to have higher symptomatic levels of depression, hopelessness, complicated grief, and anxiety, as well as lower levels of perceived social support, than nonideators at study entry. Fifty-seven percent of the patients with high complicated grief scores were found to be ideators during the follow-up versus 24% of the patients with low complicated grief scores. Patients with any suicidal ideation had higher symptom levels of depression, anxiety, and complicated grief when they were ideators as compared with periods when they denied ideation. Fifteen out of the 39 ideators had recurrent depressive episodes versus 5 of the 91 nonideators. Patients with a history of suicide attempts were more likely to be ideators after loss than other bereaved. Thus, the condition of having high levels of complicated grief symptoms and depressive symptoms appears to make bereaved individuals vulnerable to suicidal ideation. Detection of high levels of complicated grief could help clinicians identify patients who may be at heightened risk for suicide. 相似文献
902.
The pantomimic performances of a typical Broca's (nonfluent) and Wernicke's (fluent) aphasic were compared with each other and with four normal control subjects on a simple task of nonverbal referential communication. Both aphasic subjects demonstrated only about 50% accuracy in their pantomimic communication. Also, measures were obtained of the motoric fluency of the pantomimes of all subjects and comparisons were made between the fluent and nonfluent aphasics. These measures demonstrated distinct differences in the fluency patterns of the pantomimes of the two aphasic subjects similar to the differences in speech fluency which distinguish and characterize these two types of aphasia; that is, the fluent aphasic pantomimed fluently and like the control subjects and the nonfluent aphasic pantomimed nonfluently. The quantitative and qualitative similarities in the fluency patterns of the speech and the pantomimic behaviors of the aphasic subjects are discussed in terms of their implications for a definition of aphasia. 相似文献
903.
Sandra D. Mitchell 《Erkenntnis》2002,57(3):329-350
It has been claimed that ceteris paribus laws, rather than strict laws are the proper aim of the special sciences. This is so because the causal regularities found
in these domains are exception-ridden, being contingent on the presence of the appropriate conditions and the absence of interfering
factors. I argue that the ceteris paribus strategy obscures rather than illuminates the important similarities and differences between representations of causal regularities
in the exact and inexact sciences. In particular, a detailed account of the types and degrees of contingency found in the
domain of biology permits a more adequate understanding of the relations among the sciences.
This revised version was published online in July 2006 with corrections to the Cover Date. 相似文献
904.
Face recognition models suggest independent processing for functionally different types of information, such as identity, expression, sex, and facial speech. Interference between sex and expression information was tested using both a rating study and Garner's selective attention paradigm using speeded sex and expression decisions. When participants were asked to assess the masculinity of male and female angry and surprised faces, they found surprised faces to be more feminine than angry ones (Experiment 1). However, in a speeded-classification situation in the laboratory in which the sex decision was either "easy" relative to the expression decision (Experiment 2) or of more equivalent difficulty (Experiment 3), it was possible for participants to attend selectively to either dimension without interference from the other. Qualified support is offered for independent processing routes. 相似文献
905.
Wilson M 《Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association》2002,50(1):383-5; author reply 385-6
906.
In 2 experiments, young and older adults heard target speech presented in quiet or with a competing speaker in the background. The distractor consisted either of meaningful speech or nonmeaningful speech composed of randomly ordered word strings (Experiment 1) or speech in an unfamiliar language (Experiment 2). Tests of recall for the target speech showed that older adults, but not younger adults, were impaired more by meaningful distractors than by nonmeaningful distracters. However, on a surprise recognition test, young adults were more likely than older adults to recognize meaningful distractor items. These results suggest that reduced efficiency in attentional control is an important factor in older adults' difficulty in recalling target speech in the presence of a background of competing speech. 相似文献
907.
Blair RJ Mitchell DG Richell RA Kelly S Leonard A Newman C Scott SK 《Journal of abnormal psychology》2002,111(4):682-686
The processing of emotional expressions is fundamental for normal socialization and interaction. Reduced responsiveness to the expressions of sadness and fear has been implicated in the development of psychopathy (R. J. R. Blair, 1995). The current study investigates the ability of adult psychopathic individuals to process vocal affect. Psychopathic and nonpsychopathic adults, defined by the Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R; R. D. Hare, 1991), were presented with neutral words spoken with intonations conveying happiness, disgust, anger, sadness, and fear and were asked to identify the emotion of the speaker on the basis of prosody. The results indicated that psychopathic inmates were particularly impaired in the recognition of fearful vocal affect. These results are interpreted with reference to the low-fear and violence inhibition mechanism models of psychopathy. 相似文献
908.
Patricia Damery 《Psychological Perspectives》2014,57(2):147-157
This article describes my journey in learning to negotiate the shamanic realm, also referred to as the psychoid, the imaginal, or as subtle energies. Although The Red Book records Jung's work in this realm where healing occurs, analytic training too often does not emphasize the development of these spiritual tools. As a result of a series of unusual experiences, I had to seek an unconventional spiritual teacher to complete my own initiation as an analyst and then find my own way. The substance of the shamanic realm is discussed as well as placement of psyche. 相似文献
909.
Male and female black undergraduates enrolled in Introductory Psychology and Human Development courses at a predominantly black university responded to five stories about married black physicians whose total income was greatest, respectively, when the husband cared for the children (Story 1), the wife cared for the children (Story 2), either spouse cared for them (Story 3), the wife earned more than the husband (Story 4), or the husband earned more than the wife (Story 5). In contrast to the results of a study completed at a predominantly white university, black females favored maternal child care even when, as a result, the family income was reduced and had significantly less of a preference for higher relative salary for the wife than did females in the study at a predominantly white university; although black males preferred maternal child care in Story 2 and Story 3, the preference was significantly less than that of males in the study at a predominantly white university. The results were discussed in terms of their relevance to collaborating career patterns among middle-class black families. 相似文献
910.
Marilyn A. Borges Joseph R. Levine Patricia A. Naylor 《Psychology of women quarterly》1982,6(4):406-414
To ascertain the veridicality of projected attitudes toward sex role behavior, 74 male and 74 female undergraduates filled out the Attitudes Toward Women Scale (AWS) using three different targets: self, typical female classmate, and typical male classmate. Analyses of variance on respondents' total AWS scores and on the six AWS subscale scores showed significant interactions between sex of respondent and sex of target for all seven variables. Since the directions of the interactions were not consistent across subscales, the results indicated the methodological necessity of using measurements which examine a variety of specific subclasses of sex role behaviors (rather than using unidimensional scale or total test scores) when stereotypic sex role attitudes are sought. In general, both sexes were found to have a fairly accurate perception of women's attitudes, but both sexes consistently perceived the typical male as more conservative than the male self-ratings justified. 相似文献