首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   53589篇
  免费   2137篇
  国内免费   30篇
  2020年   603篇
  2019年   754篇
  2018年   1102篇
  2017年   1095篇
  2016年   1148篇
  2015年   780篇
  2014年   937篇
  2013年   4705篇
  2012年   1712篇
  2011年   1855篇
  2010年   1092篇
  2009年   1124篇
  2008年   1690篇
  2007年   1682篇
  2006年   1465篇
  2005年   1342篇
  2004年   1320篇
  2003年   1185篇
  2002年   1230篇
  2001年   1778篇
  2000年   1673篇
  1999年   1282篇
  1998年   686篇
  1997年   596篇
  1996年   535篇
  1995年   533篇
  1994年   525篇
  1993年   543篇
  1992年   1028篇
  1991年   966篇
  1990年   964篇
  1989年   871篇
  1988年   831篇
  1987年   816篇
  1986年   821篇
  1985年   866篇
  1984年   684篇
  1983年   596篇
  1982年   489篇
  1979年   663篇
  1978年   505篇
  1976年   459篇
  1975年   602篇
  1974年   624篇
  1973年   603篇
  1972年   544篇
  1971年   479篇
  1968年   544篇
  1967年   537篇
  1966年   473篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
31.
In three experiments we studied the relationship between contextual conditioning and the reinstatement of extinguished lever pressing that occurs when noncontingent food is introduced following extinction. In all three experiments the non-contingent food was presented off-baseline (with the response levers not present). On subsequent tests, with the response levers present, animals that had been exposed to food showed more reinstatement of lever pressing than control animals. This finding rules out alternative mechanisms for the reinstated responding that rely on the interaction of non-contingent food and responding, such as superstitious reinforcement or the discriminative after-effects of food. In addition, in each experiment we demonstrated that manipulations known to affect contextual conditioning (signalling the food in Experiment 1, context extinction in Experiment 2, and switching contexts in Experiment 3) reduced the reinstatement. These results are consistent with the claim that contextual conditioning is important in controlling instrumental conditioning and closely parallel findings concerning the reinstatement of Pavlovian responsing following extinction.  相似文献   
32.
33.
We investigated whether young children are able to infer affiliative relations and relative status from observing others' imitative interactions. Children watched videos showing one individual imitating another and were asked about the relationship between those individuals. Experiment 1 showed that 5‐year‐olds assume that individuals imitate people they like. Experiment 2 showed that children of the same age assume that an individual who imitates is relatively lower in status. Thus, although there are many advantages to imitating others, there may also be reputational costs. Younger children, 4‐year‐olds, did not reliably make either inference. Taken together, these experiments demonstrate that imitation conveys valuable information about third party relationships and that, at least by the age of 5, children are able to use this information in order to infer who is allied with whom and who is dominant over whom. In doing so, they add a new dimension to our understanding of the role of imitation in human social life.  相似文献   
34.
35.
36.
37.
38.
39.
By roughly 6 years of age, children acquire the stereotype that men are more competent than women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM), potentially leading to greater trust in scientific information provided by men. This study tested whether 3- to 8-year-old children differentially endorsed conflicting information about science and toys presented by male and female informants depicted as a ‘man’ and ‘woman’ (Exp1) or ‘scientists’ (Exp2). Children were expected to endorse toy testimony from gender-matched informants; thus, the key question concerned endorsement of science testimony. In Exp1 (N = 149), boys and girls showed a same-gender informant preference for toy testimony; however, girls endorsed the male informant's testimony more for science than for toys – but only when tested by a male experimenter. In Exp2 (N = 264), boys and girls showed a same-gender preference, irrespective of content. Findings suggest that STEM-related gender stereotypes might lead girls to trust scientific information presented by men over women in certain contexts.  相似文献   
40.
陈莲笙道长生于1917年10月25日,上海市人.历任中国道教协会副会长,中国道教协会顾问,上海市道教协会会长.上海市道教协会名誉会长,上海城隍庙住持,上海道学院院长,<上海道教>杂志主编,上海市宗教学会理事,上海市政协第七届委员,上海市政协第八、九届常委等职.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号