全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2221篇 |
免费 | 22篇 |
专业分类
2243篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 9篇 |
2022年 | 8篇 |
2021年 | 22篇 |
2020年 | 25篇 |
2019年 | 28篇 |
2018年 | 57篇 |
2017年 | 47篇 |
2016年 | 59篇 |
2015年 | 44篇 |
2014年 | 50篇 |
2013年 | 238篇 |
2012年 | 90篇 |
2011年 | 95篇 |
2010年 | 64篇 |
2009年 | 68篇 |
2008年 | 86篇 |
2007年 | 98篇 |
2006年 | 72篇 |
2005年 | 83篇 |
2004年 | 86篇 |
2003年 | 81篇 |
2002年 | 72篇 |
2001年 | 39篇 |
2000年 | 33篇 |
1999年 | 31篇 |
1998年 | 38篇 |
1997年 | 32篇 |
1996年 | 36篇 |
1995年 | 32篇 |
1994年 | 34篇 |
1993年 | 34篇 |
1992年 | 27篇 |
1991年 | 21篇 |
1990年 | 31篇 |
1989年 | 31篇 |
1988年 | 29篇 |
1987年 | 19篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 22篇 |
1984年 | 27篇 |
1983年 | 21篇 |
1982年 | 26篇 |
1981年 | 23篇 |
1980年 | 19篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 24篇 |
1977年 | 11篇 |
1976年 | 20篇 |
1975年 | 17篇 |
1973年 | 12篇 |
排序方式: 共有2243条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
931.
932.
Meada G. Hall M.S. John W. Schuster Ed.D. Mark Wolery Ph.D. David L. Gast Ph.D. Patricia Munson Doyle M.S. 《Journal of Behavioral Education》1992,2(3):257-279
This investigation evaluated the use of dyadic instructional arrangements in teaching chained skills to four students with moderate mental retardation. Each instructional dyad consisted of two students. Each of the three cooking skills was divided into two equal parts, and each student in a dyad received direct instruction on one part of the task analysis during each instructional session. During the following session, the two parts were reversed and a student received instruction on the second part. A constant time delay procedure was used to teach the targeted skills in an off-campus setting. A multiple probe design across skills and replicated across students evaluated the effectiveness of the instructional procedure and teaching arrangement. Results indicate that each of the four students learned the three cooking skills. Implications for future research are discussed. 相似文献
933.
Data from four reading-related tests, including a measure of knowledge about books and print conventions, and two demographic variables, obtained at the end of the first year of schooling, were used to predict a range of students' word and discourse reading achievement at the conclusion of that year. Students were enrolled in a reading program that emphasized either the subskills involved in breaking the reading code or the more holistic gaining of meaning from print. The results indicated that a measure of knowledge about books and print conventions is strongly related to end-of-year reading achievement, but that, over and above the other more traditional measures, it is a significant predictor of only one type of comprehension - that involving the inferential linking of two pieces of information separated in the text. Students in a subskills-emphasis program outperformed those in a meaning-emphasis program on tests of word knowledge but not of comprehension of discourse. 相似文献
934.
Annie Kupelian Rena E. Falk Jana Klein Patricia Fournier Nathan Fischel-Ghodsian 《Journal of genetic counseling》1996,5(1):17-26
A healthy woman sought preconceptional genetic counseling regarding a family history of a mitochondrial myopathy in her brother and retinitis pigmentosa (RP) in her two maternal aunts. Several questions were raised: (1) What is the likelihood of a familial mitochondrial condition? (2) What molecular tests or prenatal screening can we offer? (3) How would these tests help assess the likelihood of a familial mitochondrial condition? A mitochondrial mutation previously identified in the brother consisted of a heteroplasmic 2.9 kb deletion. We detected this deletion in the peripheral blood of the brother by PCR amplification of the deletion breakpoint, but not in his mother, the consultand, nor in one of the two aunts affected with RP. Although the molecular analysis was encouraging to the consultand, a familial mitochondrial disorder could not be eliminated with certainty. The pros and cons of prenatal testing for mitochondrial disorders are discussed in general, and as specifically related to this family. 相似文献
935.
We examined performance schemata in dyadic competitive and cooperative situations, that is, the ways people perceive the relationship between performance level, capacity level, and degree of motivation in situations in which two people confront or collaborate with one another. In Experiment 1, 104 participants judged the possible performance of Person A, whose ability and motivation levels were communicated, competing against Person B, whose ability and motivation levels were also communicated. When judging A's performance, participants seemed first to assess the difficulty of the task facing A and second to integrate information relative to A according to a rule with changing parameters already proposed by Surber (1981a, 1981b). In Experiment 2, 140 participants judged either the possible performance of Person A in a cooperative situation or the possible performance of the A-B team using the same material. Results showed that people judge A's performance or the team's performance using a very simple additive rule. 相似文献
936.
937.
938.
939.
940.
E. M. Elliott, Cowan, and Valle-Inclan (1998) reported a cross-modal Stroop-like interference effect in adults when an auditory distractor (a color or noncolor word) occurred simultaneously with a color patch to be named. Response times were slower with color as opposed to noncolor distractors. To distinguish two accounts of this phenomenon, we tested 4- to 11-year-olds and adults. The suppression hypothesis posits that the irrelevant word enters a phonological buffer and is injurious to color naming if the participant is unable to suppress its representation in time. The concurrent processing hypothesis states that interference occurs when the distractor and the color name are lexically accessed at the same time. Our finding that the cross-modal Stroop effect occurred in young children even with a distractor presented 500 msec in advance of the color patch favors the suppression account. Development in executive functioning may also contribute to the interference effect's becoming progressively weaker with age. 相似文献