全文获取类型
收费全文 | 778篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
782篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 5篇 |
2023年 | 6篇 |
2022年 | 8篇 |
2021年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 12篇 |
2019年 | 19篇 |
2018年 | 19篇 |
2017年 | 24篇 |
2016年 | 22篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 25篇 |
2013年 | 72篇 |
2012年 | 27篇 |
2011年 | 31篇 |
2010年 | 22篇 |
2009年 | 16篇 |
2008年 | 35篇 |
2007年 | 28篇 |
2006年 | 41篇 |
2005年 | 30篇 |
2004年 | 28篇 |
2003年 | 19篇 |
2002年 | 23篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 15篇 |
1999年 | 20篇 |
1998年 | 9篇 |
1997年 | 17篇 |
1996年 | 16篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 12篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 9篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1972年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
1967年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有782条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
Ignatius J Toner Laura P Moore Pamela K Ashley 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1978,26(1):85-91
The effect of having a child serve as a rule-following model for other children on the model's own subsequent rule-following was investigated in a resistance to deviation situation. First- and second-grade boys (age range 73 to 94 months) who were told they would serve as self-controlling models for others and who actually served in this capacity touched prohibited toys less than did boys who were not told they would serve as models. Boys told they were to serve as models but who were prevented from performing deviated at a level between the models and boys given no responsibility. Having children serve as rule-following models for others is suggested as an effective, non-punitive technique to increase their self-control. 相似文献
12.
Perceived Control, Drive for Thinness, and Food Consumption: Anorexic Tendencies as Displaced Reactance 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Although loss of perceived control has been implicated in the development of eating disorders, previous research has not directly tested the relationship between perceived control and food consumption. This study investigated the hypothesis that individuals with anorexic tendencies react to low perceived control by restricting food intake as a means of regaining a sense of control. Forty female undergraduates who scored either low or high on the Drive for Thinness Scale (Garner & Olmsted, 1984) were led to believe they would be participating in two separate studies. Perceived control was experimentally manipulated such that half of the subjects experienced low control and half experienced high control over a social situation. Under the guise of a second experiment, subjects tasted breakfast cereals and completed measures relevant to eating and body image. Results showed that subjects who were high in drive for thinness (DT) who experienced low control ate less sweetened cereal and planned to eat less at dinner than high DT subjects who experienced high control. Low DT subjects were unaffected by the control manipulation. The results are discussed in terms of displaced reactance. 相似文献
13.
14.
Sex Roles - The purpose of this study was to examine the representation of Black and Asian women coaches on women’s and men’s intercollegiate athletic teams. Through the theoretical... 相似文献
15.
Pamela K. Adelmann 《Sex roles》1993,29(3-4):195-212
Social roles have been a central focus in both sociological and developmental theories of adjustment in late life. Research on roles and well-being, however, has focused on earlier adulthood or on comparisons of tenuous roles (such as retiree) with institutional roles (such as employee), especially among men. The few studies comparing older women in the tenuous roles of homemaker and retiree all compare single-role women, and show contradictory results. One possibility is that multiple role identities are linked to greater well-being in older women, as they are among younger women. In this paper the life satisfaction, depression, and self-esteem of older women who consider themselves retired only, homemakers only, or both are compared. The sample includes 864 Black (N=242) and White (N=622) women ages 60 and over from a 1986 national survey. Retired women and homemakers did not differ in well-being; women who called themselves both had higher self-esteem and lower depression than single-role women. In analyses exploring what tenuous role content (daily activities) might explain group differences in well-being, do-it-yourself activities and volunteer activities were significant. Age, education, health, and marital status were also important factors. Recommendations include adopting a multiple role perspective with older women and encouraging their participation in instrumental, informal role activities to maximize well-being.Northwestern University 相似文献
16.
Richard L. Doty Richard Smith Donald A. Mckeown Jaya Raj 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1994,56(6):701-707
It is not known whether nominally different olfactory tests actually measure dissimilar perceptual attributes. In this study, we administered nine olfactory tests, including tests of odor identification, discrimination, detection, memory, and suprathreshold intensity and pleasantness perception, to 97 healthy subjects. Aprincipal components analysis performed on the intercorrelation matrix revealed four meaningful components. The first was comprised of strong primary loadings from most of the olfactory test measures, whereas the second was comprised of primary loadings from intensity ratings given to a set of suprathreshold odorant concentrations. The third and fourth components had primary loadings that reflected, respectively, mean suprathreshold pleasantness ratings and a response bias measure derived from a yes/no odor identification signal detection task. In an effort to adjust for potential confounding influences of age, gender, smoking, and years of schooling on the component structure, a matrix of residuals from a multiple regression analysis, which included these variables, was also analyzed. A similar component pattern emerged. Overall, these findings suggest, in healthy subjects spanning a wide age range, that (1) a number of nominally distinct tests of olfactory function are measuring a common source of variance, and (2) some suprathreshold odor intensity and pleasantness rating tests may be measuring sources of variance different from this common source. 相似文献
17.
Kelly L. Zellars Pamela L. Perrewe Wayne A. Hochwarter 《Journal of applied social psychology》1999,29(11):2250-2271
The purpose of this study was to examine the extent to which factors in the nursing environment have differential effects on burnout levels among nurses high and low in negative affectivity (NA). Specifically, this field study examined the moderating relationships among role ambiguity, role conflict, and collective efficacy on the NA–burnout relationship among nurses in a hospital setting. Findings suggest that perceived role conflict exacerbates while perceptions of collective efficacy reduce specific dimensions of burnout for nurses high in NA. These results offer some evidence regarding which environmental characteristics may mitigate burnout for high-NA nurses. 相似文献
18.
Pamela Nicely Catherine S. Tamis-LeMonda Marc H. Bornstein 《Infant behavior & development》1999,22(4):209
To investigate infants’ affective expressivity and maternal attuned responsiveness to infant expressivity in relation to early language achievement, 77 dyads were visited in their homes at 9 and 13 months, and mothers were interviewed about their children’s language between 9 and 21 months. Maternal responses that were attuned to infant affect, by selectively matching either the gradient features or the valence of infants’ affective expressions, were more predictive of children’s language achievement than maternal nonmatching responses; and maternal matching responses at 9 months were more predictive of children’s language achievements than maternal responses at 13 months. Moreover, maternal matching responses at 9 months predicted second-year language achievements over and above infant affect expressivity at 9 and 13 months, and over and above maternal matching responses at 13 months. Infants’ affective expressivity per se was not predictive. 相似文献
19.
Pamela Hanway Lucy Akehurst Zarah Vernham Lorraine Hope 《Applied cognitive psychology》2024,38(6):e70011
Investigative interviewers are often required to accurately remember information that has been provided by different people. This can be at the scene of an event or during follow-up investigations in interview rooms. Interviewers must accurately monitor the source of information to differentiate between witnesses' accounts and to assess what information is novel and what has been corroborated by others or by physical evidence. The current research examined the effects of cognitive load on memory and source monitoring accuracy for information provided by multiple witnesses. Participants, under conditions of high cognitive load (HCL) where load was induced via interviewer-relevant tasks (e.g., formulating questions) or no cognitive load (NCL), watched five mock-witnesses' accounts of the same crime. Each witness provided several details of the crime that were unique to their individual account. When asked about account details, and which witness had provided each detail, mock-interviewers' memory accuracy was lower in the HCL condition than the NCL condition. There was no difference between cognitive load conditions for source monitoring accuracy, which was poor regardless of condition. 相似文献
20.
Seong-Hyeon Kim Narae Lee Pamela Ebstyne King 《Journal for the scientific study of religion》2020,59(1):62-83
In lieu of the traditional text data analysis methods, structural topic modeling was utilized to analyze the text contents of 255 self-report inventories of religion and spirituality (R/S) published from 1929 to 2017. The study had two objectives: (a) to clarify and identify the latent dimensions of R/S inherent in the items of the measures; and (b) to examine and demonstrate the usefulness of a longitudinal topic modeling in the study of R/S. We identified 5,617 unique text terms from the measures and fitted topic models on those terms to extract latent dimensions called topics. We also simultaneously analyzed the longitudinal effect of publication decade (i.e., 1950s–2010s) on the topics. A topic model with three topics was chosen to best support the data: Experience of Transcendence (Topic 1), Engagement in Transcendence (Topic 2), and Essence of Transcendence (Topic 3). In addition, the longitudinal analysis revealed that Topic 1 showed a continual increase over the decades, while Topics 2 and 3 both demonstrated a gradual decrease, in effect matching the general trend of Topic 1's increasing popularity in society and the academia. 相似文献