首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   119篇
  免费   4篇
  123篇
  2016年   1篇
  2015年   1篇
  2013年   1篇
  2012年   1篇
  2011年   3篇
  2010年   3篇
  2009年   2篇
  2008年   6篇
  2007年   9篇
  2006年   1篇
  2005年   6篇
  2004年   2篇
  2002年   4篇
  2001年   3篇
  1999年   2篇
  1998年   2篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   5篇
  1994年   4篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   1篇
  1991年   2篇
  1990年   2篇
  1989年   3篇
  1987年   1篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   1篇
  1983年   5篇
  1982年   5篇
  1981年   1篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   3篇
  1978年   4篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   1篇
  1974年   1篇
  1973年   4篇
  1972年   2篇
  1971年   1篇
  1970年   2篇
  1969年   2篇
  1963年   1篇
  1958年   1篇
  1956年   1篇
  1953年   1篇
  1952年   1篇
  1951年   1篇
  1949年   1篇
  1937年   1篇
排序方式: 共有123条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
81.
Work sample exams are generally thought to have either low or comparatively low levels of ethnic group differences when used for personnel selection. Such exams are sometimes called “simulation exercises” and involve having applicants perform a set of tasks that are similar to those performed on the job. The nearly ubiquitous meta‐analytic value of Black–White subgroup differences in the literature is d= .38. Unfortunately, this estimate is plagued by a variety of problems (e.g., range restriction, inclusion of nonwork sample tests). Further, there are virtually no analyses that examine how the saturation of different constructs influence work sample tests. We gathered available data for Black–White ethnic group differences and found that overall work sample differences were markedly larger for samples of job applicants (d= .73) than previously thought. We also examined how different exercises and saturation of different constructs influenced work sample ds. For example, work sample test ratings of cognitive and job knowledge skills were associated with a mean observed d= .80, whereas ratings of various social skills were associated with mean observed ds that varied from .21 to .27. We urge scientists and practitioners to consider both the method and the constructs that are targeted when forecasting predictor ds.  相似文献   
82.
83.
84.
85.
86.
87.
This study explored competing normative interpretations of the dilution effect: the tendency for people to underutilize diagnostic evidence in prediction tasks when that evidence is accompanied by irrelevant information. From the normative vantage point of the intuitive statistician, the dilution effect is a judgmental bias that arises from the representativeness heuristic (similarity-matching of causes and effects). From the normative prospective of the intuitive politician, however, the dilution effect is a rational response to evidence presented in a setting in which Gricean norms of conversation are assumed to hold. The current experiment factorially manipulated conversational norms, the degree to which diagnostic evidence was diluted by irrelevant evidence, and the accountability of subjects for their judgments. Accountable subjects demonstrated a robust dilution effect when conversational norms were explicitly primed as well as in the no-priming control condition, but no dilution when conversational norms were explicitly deactivated. Non-accountable subjects demonstrated the dilution effect across norm activation conditions, with the strongest effect under the activation of conversational norms. Although the results generally support the conversational-norm interpretation of dilution, the significant dilution effect among non-accountable subjects in the norm-deactivated condition is more consistent with the judgmental-bias interpretation.  相似文献   
88.
This research describes a new approach to the measurement and enhancement of organizational productivity entitled the Productivity Measurement and Enhancement System (ProMES). Various conceptual issues regarding productivity are briefly reviewed and the system is described. Research is reported in which productivity measures using this approach were developed and evaluated in five organizational units. The measures from the system were then used to give group-level feedback, followed by group goal setting and group incentives. Results indicated that unit personnel were cooperative in developing and using the system and that the system had good psychometric characteristics. Feedback increased productivity 50%, and feedback with the addition of goal setting and incentives increased productivity 75% and 76%, respectively. The approach to measuring productivity has several unique features. For example, it combines all the functions of a unit into a single index of productivity that reflects  相似文献   
89.
The purpose of this article is to consider the ethical issues surrounding the “simplest” case of in vitro fertilization from the author's interpretation of a Catholic perspective. (Editor's Note. Any references to Catholicism or Catholic perspective refer to the author's interpretation.)  相似文献   
90.
In this research we developed and validated an interactive video assessment of conflict resolution skills. A model of conflict management was used to develop the conflict scenarios and part of the scoring key. Computer assessments of conflict resolution skills and two cognitive abilities were administered to 347 supervisors and job performance ratings were collected from their managers. The conflict skills assessment was found to be significantly related to supervisory ratings of on-the-job performance in managing conflict but to be unrelated to the measures of cognitive ability. In addition, the conflict skills assessment had no adverse impact for women. The implications of these results and directions for future research are discussed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号