首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   72691篇
  免费   3062篇
  国内免费   30篇
  2020年   876篇
  2019年   968篇
  2018年   1450篇
  2017年   1432篇
  2016年   1491篇
  2015年   1049篇
  2014年   1315篇
  2013年   6076篇
  2012年   2385篇
  2011年   2442篇
  2010年   1430篇
  2009年   1539篇
  2008年   2135篇
  2007年   2084篇
  2006年   1939篇
  2005年   1615篇
  2004年   1647篇
  2003年   1577篇
  2002年   1493篇
  2001年   2451篇
  2000年   2332篇
  1999年   1751篇
  1998年   806篇
  1997年   712篇
  1996年   760篇
  1995年   685篇
  1994年   677篇
  1993年   686篇
  1992年   1455篇
  1991年   1311篇
  1990年   1342篇
  1989年   1267篇
  1988年   1210篇
  1987年   1157篇
  1986年   1147篇
  1985年   1263篇
  1984年   1003篇
  1983年   847篇
  1982年   659篇
  1979年   1019篇
  1978年   768篇
  1975年   856篇
  1974年   965篇
  1973年   971篇
  1972年   826篇
  1971年   759篇
  1969年   666篇
  1968年   846篇
  1967年   742篇
  1966年   737篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
951.
952.
953.
Forty-eight American children, 12 popular and 12 rejected children from Grades 3 and 6, were paired with same-sex friends and nonfriends on an interactive task. Children were videotaped and their interactions judged for the amount and type of attention each child in the dyad gave to each other and the conversation. Main dependent measures included mutual engagements, acknowledgement of the partner, getting the attention of the partner, attention to the conversation, and social impact of the utterances. Both rejected and popular children attended to the conversation and the partner, but rejected children appeared to overattend in several ways. The interactions of rejected children and their matches involved more mutual engagements, conversational initiators, facilitators, terminators, and nonverbal attention-getting devices. Sex and developmental effects were also found.  相似文献   
954.
955.
The time course of phonological code activation in two writing systems   总被引:18,自引:0,他引:18  
  相似文献   
956.
957.
An assessment of the Hasher and Zacks (1979) conceptualization of the automatic nature of the acquisition of spatial location information was made in the context of manipulations of subjects' intention to learn and mood state. The Velten (1968) mood induction procedure was used to establish depressed and nondepressed subjects. Half of each group was then presented with a matrix of geometric forms under instructions to learn the location of the forms (intentional) or to evaluate the pleasantness of the array (incidental). Subsequent tests of spatial location performance found no differences as a function of subjects' mood state, but did find a strong effect for instructional condition. The superior performance of the intentional subjects raises questions about the automatic nature of the encoding of spatial location.  相似文献   
958.
A clinical example of the effects of the absence of the father during the early childhood of a male analysand and its consequences for his later object choice is presented. This patient's biological father left the household when the patient was only a few months old, and his mother did not remarry until he was five years of age. Unconscious fantasies of the lost father, with whom the patient had no contact in reality, and longing for him organized the patient's drives and determined his self-identity as well as his later object choice. The patient developed both a positive and negative identification with the lost father. The identification was with a fantasy object not a real one. The case highlights the need to use a precise term in talking of the object, namely the mental representation of the object which may or may not be built up out of experiences with the real object.  相似文献   
959.
The superego is heir to the Oedipus complex but has a much larger developmental legacy which includes preoedipal precursors and the influence of latency and adolescence. The superego continues to change in function and content throughout life, and radical transformation in adolescence may result in developmental discontinuity as well as core developmental continuity. A case is discussed in which adolescence was overlooked in previous analysis and in which adolescent superego modification had a major impact on the patient's character and his adult neurosis. The developmental significance of adolescence experienced under conditions of social isolation and rejection with forebodings of the Holocaust was unrecognized in sanctioned silence and shared analytic denial. These repeated earlier experiences of silent submission and stifled protest, and the silent suffering of the patient and his family, were an integral part of his humiliating and emasculating adolescent experiences. The intimidated adolescent, threatened from within and without, identified with the aggressor as well as with the victim. Identification with the aggressor and glorified victor contributed to a final adolescent structuralization of a punitive, sadistic superego and a rigidly perfectionistic ego ideal. As an adult, he tended to passive masochistic compliance with diminished self-esteem and unconscious self-denigration. He was prone to shame and guilt, self-criticism, and hidden hypercritical attitudes toward others. The adolescent internalization of aggression, intense castration anxiety, and pervasive narcissistic mortification led to retreat from resolution of revived oedipal conflict and to concomitant detrimental superego alteration. These issues were of major importance for analytic understanding and therapeutic progress.  相似文献   
960.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号