首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   41809篇
  免费   1697篇
  国内免费   52篇
  2020年   429篇
  2019年   483篇
  2018年   762篇
  2017年   736篇
  2016年   779篇
  2015年   543篇
  2014年   694篇
  2013年   3207篇
  2012年   1181篇
  2011年   1284篇
  2010年   730篇
  2009年   793篇
  2008年   1058篇
  2007年   1128篇
  2006年   1006篇
  2005年   906篇
  2004年   844篇
  2003年   821篇
  2002年   822篇
  2001年   1434篇
  2000年   1378篇
  1999年   1056篇
  1998年   433篇
  1997年   390篇
  1996年   399篇
  1992年   872篇
  1991年   824篇
  1990年   847篇
  1989年   758篇
  1988年   761篇
  1987年   704篇
  1986年   740篇
  1985年   787篇
  1984年   622篇
  1983年   541篇
  1982年   408篇
  1981年   385篇
  1979年   654篇
  1978年   493篇
  1977年   405篇
  1975年   550篇
  1974年   683篇
  1973年   696篇
  1972年   542篇
  1971年   507篇
  1970年   464篇
  1969年   461篇
  1968年   610篇
  1967年   539篇
  1966年   533篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
91.
92.
93.
94.
95.
96.
Although it is currently popular to model human associative learning using connectionist networks, the mechanism by which their output activations are converted to probabilities of response has received relatively little attention. Several possible models of this decision process are considered here, including a simple ratio rule, a simple difference rule, their exponential versions, and a winner-take-all network. Two categorization experiments that attempt to dissociate these models are reported. Analogues of the experiments were presented to a single-layer, feed-forward, delta-rule network. Only the exponential ratio rule and the winner-take-all architecture, acting on the networks' output activations that corresponded to responses available on test, were capable of fully predicting the mean response results. In addition, unlike the exponential ratio rule, the winner-take-all model has the potential to predict latencies. Further studies will be required to determine whether latencies produced under more stringent conditions conform to the model's predictions.  相似文献   
97.
98.
When forecasting future outcomes, people tend to believe that the outcomes they want to happen are also likely to happen. Despite numerous attempts, few systematic factors have been identified that consistently and robustly reduce wishful thinking (WT) effects. Using elections and sporting event outcomes as contexts, three experiments examined whether taking the perspective of a political rival or opposing fan reduced WT effects. We also examined whether making deliberative (vs. intuitive-based) forecasts was associated with lower WT effects. Online adult samples of U.S. citizens from Mechanical Turk and U.S. college students provided their preferences and forecasts for the U.S. presidential election (Experiments 1 and 2) and a sports competition outcome (Experiment 3). Critically, some participants received perspective taking prompts immediately before providing forecasts. First, results revealed reductions in WT effects when participants engaged in perspective taking. Interestingly, this effect only emerged when intuitive-based forecasts were made first (Experiment 3). Second, intuitive-based forecasts revealed stronger evidence of WT effects. Finally, we found that perspective taking and forming forecasts deliberately promoted a shift in focus away from preferences and toward a consideration of the relative strengths and weaknesses of the entities (i.e., candidates and teams). Theoretical implications for understanding WT effects and applied implications for developing interventions are discussed.  相似文献   
99.
睡眠不足会对人的认知、情感和人际交互产生诸多影响。这种影响在社会情绪层面表现为个体情绪共情和认知共情的减少,易激惹性与愤怒情绪的增加;在社会行为层面则表现为亲社会行为的减少和攻击行为的增加。在睡眠不足状态下,情绪系统和认知系统功能连接的减弱可能是这些变化的潜在机制。未来应结合生态效度较高的睡眠操作手段,系统考察睡眠不足如何导致各种高级社会情绪的改变,以及这些社会情绪的变化如何导致社会行为的变化。  相似文献   
100.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号