全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4583篇 |
免费 | 123篇 |
国内免费 | 13篇 |
专业分类
4719篇 |
出版年
2017年 | 7篇 |
2012年 | 448篇 |
2011年 | 525篇 |
2010年 | 125篇 |
2009年 | 52篇 |
2008年 | 434篇 |
2007年 | 471篇 |
2006年 | 390篇 |
2005年 | 341篇 |
2004年 | 284篇 |
2003年 | 297篇 |
2002年 | 260篇 |
2001年 | 166篇 |
2000年 | 235篇 |
1999年 | 83篇 |
1998年 | 40篇 |
1997年 | 23篇 |
1996年 | 13篇 |
1995年 | 14篇 |
1994年 | 17篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 12篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 14篇 |
1987年 | 12篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 16篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 13篇 |
1978年 | 14篇 |
1977年 | 11篇 |
1976年 | 13篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
1959年 | 9篇 |
1958年 | 31篇 |
1957年 | 20篇 |
1956年 | 19篇 |
1955年 | 26篇 |
1954年 | 13篇 |
1953年 | 20篇 |
1952年 | 22篇 |
1951年 | 18篇 |
1950年 | 17篇 |
1948年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有4719条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
981.
Mecklenbräuker S Steffens MC Jelenec P Goergens NK 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2011,108(4):747-761
Action–object phrases (e.g., “lift the bottle”) are remembered better if they have been enacted rather than learned verbally. This enactment effect is largest in free recall for phrases with objects (e.g., “bottle”) present because these phrases can be interactively encoded with those context objects (interactive context integration) that serve as retrieval cues. The current study investigated whether 6- and 8-year-olds are already capable of interactive context integration. Experiment 1 demonstrated interactive context integration with 8-year-olds. This was hindered in a condition where attention was directed away from context objects. Experiment 2 demonstrated interactive context integration with 6-year-old kindergartners. Taken together, our findings show that even 6-year-olds are capable of incidental context integration through enactment and that this process is attention based. 相似文献
982.
983.
C Baeken P Van Schuerbeek R De Raedt MA Vanderhasselt J De Mey A Bossuyt R Luypaert 《Brain and cognition》2012,80(2):230-236
The amygdalae are key players in the processing of a variety of emotional stimuli. Especially aversive visual stimuli have been reported to attract attention and activate the amygdalae. However, as it has been argued that passively viewing withdrawal-related images could attenuate instead of activate amygdalae neuronal responses, its role under passive viewing conditions remains unclear. Furthermore, because individual sensitivity to stress reactions has been shown to modulate amygdalae processing, the aim of the current event-related fMRI study was to investigate whether individual differences in stress proneness could influence amygdala responses while passively viewing withdrawal and approach-related visual images. We presented 14 healthy female subjects with a random sequence of images of happy 'healthy' baby faces (approach-related) and baby faces disfigured by severe dermatological conditions (withdrawal-related). No instructions were given other than to watch the images attentively. We integrated individual perceived stress (PSS) scores in our analysis. The processing of withdrawal-related pictures resulted in less left amygdala activity in females scoring higher on perceived stress. Our findings suggest that stress-sensitive healthy females are less able to fully attend to withdrawal-related visual material and in essence avoid exposure to such images in an effort to reduce strong psychophysiological responses. Although the relatively small number of participants limits drawing firm conclusions, we suggest that in passive viewing emotional brain imaging paradigms, individual information on stress proneness should be included in the interpretation of amygdala neuronal processes. 相似文献
984.
985.
在目前通用的几本哲学教材中,一般都把量变的基本形式分为两种:一是数量的增减引起的事物质变;一是由构成事物的成分在空间关系即排列次序和结构形式上的变化而引起的质变。(见肖前等主编的《辩证唯物主义原理》,第186页;李秀林等主编的 相似文献
986.
987.
在当代世界上,呈现出一幅色彩和倾向各异的形形色色的思想流派林立的图面。其中,在调查分析科技革命与社会之间越来越深刻而复杂的关系过程中出现的专家政治理论占有重要地位。为了揭示科技革命对现代社会里的各种政治制度产生何种后果,专家政治论建立了所谓“非政治化”和“非意识形态化”的学说。诚然,“非意识形态化”的论点是由不同流派的 相似文献
988.
Gustavsson-Lilius M Julkunen J Keskivaara P Lipsanen J Hietanen P 《Psychology & health》2012,27(2):178-195
Aim: The aim of this study was to clarify the associations between sense of coherence (SOC), dispositional optimism and distress (i.e. anxiety and depression) in cancer patients and their partners. Methods: The associations between SOC, dispositional optimism (Life Orientation Test-Revised, LOT-R), depression (Beck Depression Inventory-14, BDI-14) and anxiety (Endler Multidimensional Anxiety Scales, EMAS-State) were studied in 147 cancer couples. The data were collected with self-report questionnaires at the time of diagnosis (2 months) and after 6 months. Path analysis was used to analyse the predictors of follow-up distress and crossover effects in the longitudinal data. Results: Optimistic patients and patients with strong SOC as well as their partners reported fewer symptoms of depression and anxiety than less optimistic subjects and subjects with weaker SOC. Optimism partially explained the effect of SOC on distress and SOC seemed to be an independent factor in predicting distress. Patient and partner distress at baseline and at 8-month follow-up correlated positively. In addition, high partner optimism at baseline seemed to predict low patient anxiety at follow-up. Conclusions: The beneficial effects of SOC seem to include also other elements beyond optimism. In clinical practice, enhancing optimistic expectations of the future and promoting SOC could be expected to reduce distress in cancer patients and their partners. 相似文献
989.
When a horizontally elongated surface is occluded in the middle by a larger surface, it appears narrower than its true width
(Kanizsa’s compression illusion). We report that a similar compression effect occurs for closed-loop visuomotor matches of
size, but not for otherwise comparable open-loop “mimed” reaching or size-matching visuomotor responses. Our study is the
first in which a comparison of size perception in personal space with bilateral actions performed with both hands (instead
of precision grips employing the thumb and the index finger) is used to investigate motor responses to Kanizsa’s compression
illusion. Implications for the current debate on the existence of dissociations between spatial perception and visually controlled
actions in personal space are discussed. 相似文献
990.
Ferré P 《The Spanish journal of psychology》2002,5(2):78-89
Emotional stimuli are better remembered and recognized than neutral ones. This advantage for emotional stimuli has been repeatedly obtained when testing long-term retention. However, there are contradictory results concerning retention of emotional information when short retention intervals are used. The aim of the present study was, on the one hand, to test the effect of retention interval on memory for emotional stimuli (Experiment 1). The results showed that emotional information is better remembered than neutral information in both immediate and delayed memory tests, suggesting that the advantage for emotional information is not limited to long retention intervals. On the other hand, I tried to test the proposals made by Christianson and Nilsson (1984) and Bower (1992). These authors suggested that the advantage for emotional stimuli could be explained as emotional stimuli spending more processing capacity during acquisition, thus rendering less capacity available to encode simultaneously presented information (Experiments 2 and 3). Results showed that concurrent presentation of emotional stimuli did not inhibit the recall of neutral stimuli. These findings do not seem to support the proposals of Christianson and Nilsson (1984) and Bower (1992). According to these results, some mechanisms other than a greater spending of processing capacity have to be involved in the advantage for emotional information in memory. 相似文献