全文获取类型
收费全文 | 48471篇 |
免费 | 949篇 |
国内免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
49427篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 582篇 |
2019年 | 612篇 |
2018年 | 1020篇 |
2017年 | 975篇 |
2016年 | 1020篇 |
2015年 | 695篇 |
2014年 | 869篇 |
2013年 | 4005篇 |
2012年 | 1603篇 |
2011年 | 1673篇 |
2010年 | 979篇 |
2009年 | 956篇 |
2008年 | 1379篇 |
2007年 | 1381篇 |
2006年 | 1224篇 |
2005年 | 1101篇 |
2004年 | 963篇 |
2003年 | 946篇 |
2002年 | 949篇 |
2001年 | 1486篇 |
2000年 | 1458篇 |
1999年 | 1097篇 |
1998年 | 471篇 |
1997年 | 415篇 |
1996年 | 427篇 |
1992年 | 896篇 |
1991年 | 850篇 |
1990年 | 873篇 |
1989年 | 783篇 |
1988年 | 782篇 |
1987年 | 722篇 |
1986年 | 747篇 |
1985年 | 801篇 |
1984年 | 644篇 |
1983年 | 556篇 |
1982年 | 420篇 |
1979年 | 668篇 |
1978年 | 506篇 |
1977年 | 409篇 |
1976年 | 394篇 |
1975年 | 560篇 |
1974年 | 692篇 |
1973年 | 705篇 |
1972年 | 547篇 |
1971年 | 514篇 |
1970年 | 472篇 |
1969年 | 467篇 |
1968年 | 621篇 |
1967年 | 544篇 |
1966年 | 543篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
A D Pick C F Palmer B L Hennessy M G Unze R K Jones R M Richardson 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1988,45(1):28-51
Children's perception of scale and contour in melodies was investigated in five studies. Experimental tasks included judging transposed renditions of melodies (Studies 1 and 3), discriminating between transposed renditions of a melody (Study 2), judging contour-preserving transformations of melodies (Study 4), and judging similarity to a familiar target melody of transformations preserving rhythm or rhythm and contour (Study 5). The first and second studies showed that young children detect key transposition changes even in familiar melodies and they perceive similarity over key transpositions even in unfamiliar melodies. Young children also are sensitive to melodic contour over transformations that preserve it (Study 5), yet they distinguish spontaneously between melodies with the same contour and different intervals (Study 4). The key distance effect reported in the literature did not occur in the tasks of this investigation (Studies 1 and 3), and it may be apparent only for melodies shorter or more impoverished than those used here. 相似文献
12.
Using a multi-trial, free-recall paradigm (Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test; Lezak, 1983), the acquisition and retention by learning disabled (LD) young adults with a Halstead Impairment Index less than or equal to 0.3 (low LD) or greater than or equal to 0.4 (high LD) was compared with that of nondisabled and head-injured (HI) young adults. Significant group differences, particularly during the last three acquisition trials, confirmed the effectiveness of this method in revealing subtle learning and retention deficits. Analyses of Trial 1 recall showed group differences in the prerecency but not the recency portion of the serial position curve, thus implicating long-term but not short-term memory differences between the groups. Analyses of pair frequency (Sternberg & Tulving, 1977) data showed that the three clinical groups had difficulty organizing the to-be-learned material for effective retrieval. Following an interpolated task, retention was poorer for the clinical groups than for the nondisabled and varied inversely with the degree of neuropsychological impairment. Finally, during retention, the nondisabled and low LD groups retained and used the list organization established during acquisition whereas the high LD and HI groups did not. 相似文献
13.
Schinke SP Orlandi MA Botvin GJ Gilchrist LD Trimble JE Locklear VS 《Journal of counseling psychology》1988,35(1):87-90
Tobacco, alcohol, and drug use are problems for American-Indian people. We reviewed these problems and the explanations for them and described a bicultural competence skills approach for preventing substance abuse with American-Indian adolescents. Data from a study of that approach suggest its efficacy with American-Indian youth. At posttest and a 6-month follow-up, American-Indian subjects who received preventive intervention based on bicultural competence skills concepts improved more than did American-Indian subjects in a no-intervention control condition on measures of substance-use knowledge, attitudes, and interactive skills, and on self-reported rates of tobacco, alcohol, and drug use. Our findings have implications for future substance-abuse prevention research with American-Indian people. 相似文献
14.
An Expanded Attributional Style Questionnaire 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
15.
16.
Amnesia as a consequence of male rape: a case report 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
17.
18.
V Di Lollo P Dixon 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1988,14(4):671-681
Iconic memory, which was initially regarded as a unitary phenomenon, has since been subdivided into several components. In the present work we examined the joint effects of two such components (visible persistence and the visual analog representation) on performance in a partial report task. The display consisted of 15 alphabetic characters arranged around the perimeter of an imaginary circle on the face of an oscilloscope. The observer named the character singled out by a bar-probe. Two factors were varied: exposure duration of the array (10, 50, 100, 150, 200, 300, 400 or 500 ms) and duration of blank period (interstimulus interval, ISI) between the termination of the array and the onset of the probe (0, 50, 100, 150, or 200 ms). Performance was progressively impaired as both exposure duration and ISI were increased. The results were explained in terms of a probabilistic combinatorial model in which the timecourses of visible persistence and of the visual analog representation are regarded as time-locked to the onset and to the end of stimulation, respectively. The impairing effect of exposure duration was attributed to the relatively high spatial demands of the task that could be met optimally by information in visible persistence (which declines as a function of exposure duration), but less adequately by information in the visual analog representation. A second experiment, employing a task with lesser spatial demands, confirmed this interpretation. 相似文献
19.
20.
We studied the effect of different postoperative times on the behavioral recovery following brain implants. Adult male rats received cortical tissue grafts 2 weeks after aspiration of the medial frontal cortex. Either 2 (Immediate Group) or 28 (Delay Group) days after grafting, the performance of these rats on a behavioral battery, comprising the Morris water maze task, forepaw use, and grooming, was compared to that of rats with similar lesions and postoperative recovery times but no grafts. Rats tested immediately after receiving implants performed better on the spatial navigation task than rats with similar lesions but no grafts. This improvement, however, was less than that shown by rats with lesions but no grafts permitted to recover for 28 days before testing. In contrast, in the Delay Group, rats with grafts were more greatly impaired than were their operated controls. Neither lesions nor grafts affected grooming although the Immediate Group with grafts were significantly more impaired in using their forepaws during feeding than were any of the other groups. These results lead us to conclude that differing postoperative recovery times and task requirements may account for some of the inconsistent results of the influence of brain grafts on behavioral recovery reported in the literature. We also conclude that cortical tissue implants can have two effects with different time courses and opposite net behavioral effects. 相似文献