全文获取类型
收费全文 | 832篇 |
免费 | 13篇 |
专业分类
845篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 9篇 |
2022年 | 8篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 22篇 |
2019年 | 20篇 |
2018年 | 29篇 |
2017年 | 24篇 |
2016年 | 34篇 |
2015年 | 21篇 |
2014年 | 15篇 |
2013年 | 86篇 |
2012年 | 32篇 |
2011年 | 49篇 |
2010年 | 20篇 |
2009年 | 29篇 |
2008年 | 40篇 |
2007年 | 34篇 |
2006年 | 29篇 |
2005年 | 20篇 |
2004年 | 24篇 |
2003年 | 19篇 |
2002年 | 14篇 |
2001年 | 17篇 |
2000年 | 17篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 9篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 15篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 13篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1972年 | 4篇 |
1970年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 9篇 |
1967年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有845条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
551.
Two studies examined the existence, within an achievement-related context, of a social norm favoring internal explanations for task performances. In the first study, we investigated the reactions of observers to an actor's high, moderate, or low self-attribution of causal responsibility for his negative performance outcome on an ostensibly standardized aptitude test. The results indicated that the actor was evaluated more positively to the degree that he accepted more personal responsibility for his performance. In the second study, we examined the reactions of depressed and nondepressed observers to an actor's high or low self-attributions of causal responsibility for his poor performance on a test of analytical ability. On the basis of the notion that the chronic lack of control and resultant uncertainty, presumably characteristic of depressed persons, motivates attributional information processing, we expected depressed observers to be more sensitive to the actor's violation of the norm of internality and to respond with more social disapproval than nondepressed observers. Results generally were consistent with this reasoning. Experimental findings are discussed in terms of the interpersonal implications of expressed attributions. 相似文献
552.
Emotional stimuli receive high processing priority in attention and memory. This processing "advantage" is generally thought to be predominantly mediated by arousal. However, recent data suggest that ratings of an image's affective "impact" may be a better predictor of recollection than arousal or valence. One interpretation of these findings is that high-impact images may draw an individual's attention, thus facilitating subsequent processing. We investigated the allocation of visual attention to negative emotional images that differed in impact but were matched for valence, arousal, and other characteristics. Participants viewed a central image flanked by 2 neutral indoor or outdoor scenes and made speeded judgments about whether the neutral scenes matched. In Experiment 1, responses were slower on high-impact relative to low-impact or neutral trials. In Experiment 2, responses on high-arousal relative to low-arousal trials did not differ significantly. These data provide evidence for differential allocation of attention to distinct sets of negative, equally arousing images, and argue against the prevailing view that heightened attention to and processing of emotional stimuli relate simply to arousal or valence. 相似文献
553.
Esther Menaker Ph.D. Philip Turner Ph.D. Todd DuBose M.Div. Robert Lee Hill M.Div. Jennifer Dubose M.S. Claude Barbre David Asomaning M.Div. Michael W. McCann M.Div. Mark J. Hanson Ph.D. Akintunde E. Akinade Jill Carlen Kirby C.S.W. Bradley Clough Alexander Ulanov Barry Ulanov 《Journal of religion and health》1995,34(3):253-272
554.
Carrasco Ortiz MA Delgado Egido B Barbero García MI Holgado Tello FP del Barrio Gándara MV 《Psicothema》2011,23(4):824-831
This article analyses the dimensions of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis, 1980). The sample comprised 721 Spanish participants between 9 and 16 years old. Diverse exploratory factor analyses were conducted as the basis for the final confirmatory factor analysis, through self-report. The results supported a structure with 5 first-order main factors (Intellectual empathy, Positive emotional empathy, Disorganized emotional empathy, Virtual empathy, and Impassiveness), where the two first factors are nested in a second-order dimension: "Considerate Social Style". The psychometric characteristics of the instrument were adequate. The proposed model is a new alternative to conceptualize the factor structure of empathy. 相似文献
555.
Treatment guidelines recommend evidence-based guided self-help (GSH) as the first stage of treatment for bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder. The current randomised control trial evaluated a cognitive behavioural therapy-based GSH pack, ‘Working to Overcome Eating Difficulties,’ delivered by trained mental health professionals in 6 sessions over 3 months. It was congruent with the transdiagnostic approach and so was intended as suitable for all disordered eating, except severe anorexia nervosa. Eighty one clients were randomly allocated to either a GSH or waiting list condition. Eating disorder psychopathology (EDE-Q), key behavioural features and global distress (CORE) were measured at pre- and post-intervention, and 3- and 6-month follow-up. Results showed significant improvements in eating disorder psychopathology, laxative abuse, exercise behaviours, and global distress, with the GSH condition being superior to the waiting list on all outcomes. Treatment gains were maintained at 3 and 6 months. This study adds to the evidence supporting GSH for disordered eating, including EDNOS. However, further work is needed to establish the factors that contribute to observed therapeutic improvements and determine for whom GSH is most suitable. 相似文献
556.
557.
PD Dr. Andreas Hill 《Psychotherapeut》2011,56(6):475-484
The Internet shows several more or less specific characteristics, in particular accessibility, anonymity, affordability, worldwide connectivity and interactivity. These characteristics hold chances as well as risks for the development of sexuality and sexual relationships. Rarely stressed and investigated are the possible positive effects, such as easier sexual contacts especially for individuals with social deficits, for sexual minorities (e.g. gay, lesbian, transgender, sadomasochists or fetishists), reducing sexual prejudices and stereotypes, better partner matching and easy access to reliable information and counseling platforms about sexual issues. The psychotherapist more often encounters the problematic effects of Internet pornography and cybersex, such as avoiding real life (offline) sexual and non-sexual social contacts, the stress on relationships (e.g. through cyber infidelity), intensifying fantasies and impulses that are harmful to oneself or others up to criminal behavior (e.g. use and circulation of child pornography). Describing a case history this review discusses the role of the Internet in the development of sexual addictive or compulsive symptomatology, including treatment options. Only a differentiated analysis of the chances and risks can counterbalance the increasing tendencies to demonize and pathologize the use of the Internet for sexual purposes. 相似文献
558.
Developmental coordination disorder (DCD) affects around 5% of the population and is diagnosed on the basis of poor motor
coordination. Although we know rather little about the lifetime consequences of this disorder, it is clear that significant
difficulties remain through adolescence and into adulthood for the majority. It is also clear that significant psychosocial
consequences exist for many individuals with DCD. In the current study, quality of life satisfaction was investigated in a
group of emerging adults with and without DCD using the Quality of Life Satisfaction Questionnaire (Endicott et al. Psychopharmacology
Bulletin, 29, 321–326, 1993). Overall, the group of adults with DCD reported significantly lower levels of quality of life satisfaction across all domains
on the scale. This finding has important implications for consideration of early and later intervention for these individuals,
as well as for studies to consider the risk and protective factors at play in long-term outcome with respect to both the motor
skills and psychosocial aspects of this disorder. 相似文献
559.
560.
The goal of this study was to determine whether intensive training can ameliorate cognitive skills in children. Children aged 7 to 9 from low socioeconomic backgrounds participated in one of two cognitive training programs for 60 minutes/day and 2 days/week, for a total of 8 weeks. Both training programs consisted of commercially available computerized and non-computerized games. Reasoning training emphasized planning and relational integration; speed training emphasized rapid visual detection and rapid motor responses. Standard assessments of reasoning ability - the Test of Non-Verbal Intelligence (TONI-3) and cognitive speed (Coding B from WISC IV) - were administered to all children before and after training. Neither group was exposed to these standardized tests during training. Children in the reasoning group improved substantially on TONI (Cohen's d = 1.51), exhibiting an average increase of 10 points in Performance IQ, but did not improve on Coding. By contrast, children in the speed group improved substantially on Coding (d = 1.15), but did not improve on TONI. Counter to widespread belief, these results indicate that both fluid reasoning and processing speed are modifiable by training. 相似文献