全文获取类型
收费全文 | 17916篇 |
免费 | 365篇 |
国内免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
18288篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 141篇 |
2020年 | 247篇 |
2019年 | 263篇 |
2018年 | 369篇 |
2017年 | 371篇 |
2016年 | 391篇 |
2015年 | 301篇 |
2014年 | 329篇 |
2013年 | 1381篇 |
2012年 | 590篇 |
2011年 | 671篇 |
2010年 | 391篇 |
2009年 | 364篇 |
2008年 | 503篇 |
2007年 | 476篇 |
2006年 | 419篇 |
2005年 | 344篇 |
2004年 | 356篇 |
2003年 | 330篇 |
2002年 | 333篇 |
2001年 | 703篇 |
2000年 | 707篇 |
1999年 | 469篇 |
1998年 | 185篇 |
1997年 | 183篇 |
1996年 | 144篇 |
1995年 | 138篇 |
1993年 | 140篇 |
1992年 | 362篇 |
1991年 | 324篇 |
1990年 | 346篇 |
1989年 | 334篇 |
1988年 | 320篇 |
1987年 | 289篇 |
1986年 | 311篇 |
1985年 | 298篇 |
1984年 | 236篇 |
1983年 | 200篇 |
1979年 | 192篇 |
1978年 | 165篇 |
1975年 | 189篇 |
1974年 | 249篇 |
1973年 | 199篇 |
1972年 | 204篇 |
1971年 | 169篇 |
1970年 | 151篇 |
1969年 | 198篇 |
1968年 | 202篇 |
1967年 | 161篇 |
1966年 | 183篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
871.
Further evidence for feature correlations in semantic memory. 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
K McRae G S Cree R Westmacott V R de Sa 《Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale》1999,53(4):360-373
The role of feature correlations in semantic memory is a central issue in conceptual representation. In two versions of the feature verification task, participants were faster to verify that a feature (< is juicy >) is part of a concept (grapefruit) if it is strongly rather than weakly intercorrelated with the other features of that concept. Contrasting interactions between feature correlations and SOA were found when the concept versus the feature was presented first. An attractor network model of word meaning that naturally learns and uses feature correlations predicted those interactions. This research provides further evidence that semantic memory includes implicitly learned statistical knowledge of feature relationships, in contrast to theories such as spreading activation networks, in which feature correlations play no role. 相似文献
872.
Louise P. Parkes Sherry K. Schneider & Stephen Bochner 《Asian Journal of Social Psychology》1999,2(3):367-383
The cultural constructs of individualism and collectivism (I-C) have been closely identified with different conceptions of the self. In contrast to autonomous, abstract self-concepts, it has been argued that collectivists have concepts of self which are interdependent with others and connected with particular contexts. This study investigated the extent to which spontaneous social and contextual self-concepts were connected to each other and to individual-level I-C measures. Questionnaires were administered to adult employee samples in Australia and South-East Asia (N = 581). Although closely intertwined, social and contextual statements were able to be independently tested in relation to I-C. I-C was primarily related to the social-autonomous dimension of self. Controlling for social self-responses, contextual self-concepts were not independently related to any of the measures of I-C. 相似文献
873.
Age and individual differences influence prospective memory 总被引:9,自引:0,他引:9
The influence of age and individual ability differences on event-based prospective memory was examined using an adapted version of G. O. Einstein and M. A. McDaniel's (1990) task. Two samples of younger and older adults who differed in educational attainment, occupational status, and verbal ability were compared. Results yield comparable prospective performance for the younger groups and higher ability older adults; lower ability older adults performed more poorly by comparison. Hierarchical regression analyses indicated that working memory span and recognition accounted for small but significant proportions of variance in prospective performance. The contribution of ability level to prospective memory remained significant even after statistically controlling for self-reported health and social activity characteristics. Implications for current views on prospective memory aging are discussed. 相似文献
874.
K W Fulford 《Journal of abnormal psychology》1999,108(3):412-420
Nine variations on the theme of J. C. Wakefield's (1999) evolutionary definition of dysfunction show that the concept is not, as he claims, purely causal. It depends also on a teleological element of meaning introduced, in Wakefield's formulation, through an equivocation on the sense in which natural selection explains biological forms. The corollary (presented here briefly as a coda to the theme and variations) is that Wakefield's definition is not, as he also claims, value free. However, contra S. O. Lilienfeld and L. Marino (1995), this does not place diagnostic judgments of dysfunction outside the scope of science. 相似文献
875.
876.
Manas K. Mandal Yukihide Ida Susumu Harizuka Neelam Upadhaya 《International journal of psychology》1999,34(1):59-66
Indian (N = 400) and Japanese (N = 502) adult subjects were examined for their hand preference on a 7-point scale (1 = left always, 7 = right always) of the 32-item Waterloo Handedness Questionnaire (Steenhuis & Bryden, 1989). Factor analyses of the data yielded a two-factorial structure of hand preference: skilled and unskilled. Interaction of Culture × Hand preference indicated that Indians had more right-hand preference for unskilled activities whereas Japanese subjects had more right-hand preference for skilled activities. Further analyses revealed that the frequency of middle category responses was more common in Japanese subjects. Indian subjects were found to give more extreme responses for either right- or left-hand preference. Findings are discussed in the light of cultural training given to the individuals in these two societies, which are essentially collective in nature. 相似文献
877.
Aunola K Nurmi JE Onatsu-Arvilommi T Pulkkinen L 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》1999,40(4):307-317
In order to examine the extent to which parents' levels of education, financial resources, self-esteem, and their mastery-orientation versus task-avoidance are associated with their parenting styles and parental stress, data from two studies were analyzed. In Study I, parents of 105 6 to 7-year old children were asked to fill in scales measuring their parenting styles and parental stress, mastery-orientation, financial resources, and their level of education. In Study II, 235 parents were asked to fill in the same scales. An identical pattern of results was found in the two studies. Parents' self-esteem and their use of mastery-oriented strategy were found to be associated with authoritative parenting and low parental stress, whereas parents' low level of education was related to an authoritarian parenting style. The results further showed that the impact of parents' self-esteem on authoritative parenting and parental stress was partly mediated by their use of a mastery-oriented strategy. 相似文献
878.
879.
LUKAS BÖÖK 《Synthese》1999,118(1):13-30
Representationalism in cognitive science holds that semantic meaning should be explained by representations in the mind or brain. In this paper it is argued that semantic meaning should instead be explained by an abstract theory of semantic machines -- machines with predicative capability. The concept of a semantic machine (like that of a Turing machine or of Dennett's intentional systems) is not a physical concept -- although it has physical implementations. The predicative competence of semantic machines is defined in terms of independent agreement alone (cf. independent, and yet synchronised, clocks). Abstract theories are analysed as systems of quasi-apriori rules for abstract predicates. A relatively limited number of such theories and a few fundamental dimensions (space, time, mass, etc.) are today assumed to exhaust physical reality. However, that assumption need not be in conflict with predicates that cannot be defined in physical terms – for instance the functional and intentional terms that are crucial for cognitive science. 相似文献
880.
We assessed the academic performance of a 14-year-old boy with insect phobia in the context of feared stimuli. The dependent measure was math calculation rate across three conditions that varied therapist statements about the presence of crickets and the actual presence of live crickets. Subsequent treatment consisted of graduated exposure and contingent rewards for math problem completion. Assessment results indicated that the boy's performance was consistently low in the presence of live crickets but not when he was spuriously informed that crickets were present (the primary referral concern). Treatment results indicated no effect from exposure alone and a dramatic effect when exposure was combined with contingent rewards. 相似文献