全文获取类型
收费全文 | 40654篇 |
免费 | 1565篇 |
国内免费 | 16篇 |
专业分类
42235篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 481篇 |
2019年 | 592篇 |
2018年 | 823篇 |
2017年 | 882篇 |
2016年 | 889篇 |
2015年 | 609篇 |
2014年 | 737篇 |
2013年 | 3175篇 |
2012年 | 1353篇 |
2011年 | 1333篇 |
2010年 | 871篇 |
2009年 | 840篇 |
2008年 | 1201篇 |
2007年 | 1222篇 |
2006年 | 1080篇 |
2005年 | 958篇 |
2004年 | 900篇 |
2003年 | 838篇 |
2002年 | 850篇 |
2001年 | 1257篇 |
2000年 | 1191篇 |
1999年 | 924篇 |
1998年 | 401篇 |
1997年 | 400篇 |
1996年 | 375篇 |
1993年 | 376篇 |
1992年 | 784篇 |
1991年 | 696篇 |
1990年 | 746篇 |
1989年 | 649篇 |
1988年 | 686篇 |
1987年 | 645篇 |
1986年 | 645篇 |
1985年 | 586篇 |
1984年 | 547篇 |
1983年 | 514篇 |
1982年 | 384篇 |
1979年 | 617篇 |
1978年 | 445篇 |
1977年 | 383篇 |
1975年 | 506篇 |
1974年 | 549篇 |
1973年 | 576篇 |
1972年 | 439篇 |
1971年 | 434篇 |
1970年 | 377篇 |
1969年 | 432篇 |
1968年 | 514篇 |
1967年 | 479篇 |
1966年 | 392篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
211.
212.
213.
214.
This article presents a model of how attractiveness influences adjustment and reviews the relevant evidence. The model suggests that attractiveness influences adjustment through a three-stage process. Varying levels of physical attractiveness create differential responses in other people (Stage 1). These differential responses in others as a function of a person's appearance then result in differential treatment of the person (Stage 2), with such differential treatment eventually resulting in differential adjustment (Stage 3). These three stages are considered to occur in a reciprocal manner across the life span. Empirical support is found for each stage from early childhood to adulthood. Attractiveness accounts for 6% to 16% of the variance in measures of subjective adjustment as well as sociometric and behavioral measures of interpersonal adjustment. 相似文献
215.
Takeo Watanabe George L. Zimmerman Patrick Cavanagh 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1992,52(2):161-166
We examined whether the orientation-contingent color aftereffect (the McCollough effect) could be mediated by subjective horizontal and vertical structure induced by the perception of transparency. In our experiments, red vertical bars and green horizontal bars were alternated as an adapting stimulus. After adaptation, subjects (n=6) were asked to adjust the green and red saturation of a test pattern until they obtained a neutral gray. Horizontal and vertical stripes were combined in the test pattern in three different ways: (1) overlapping with a luminance combination that gave rise to a perception of transparent overlays of horizontal and vertical stripes (valid transparency condition), (2) overlapping with luminance combinations that did not induce a perception of transparency (invalid transparency condition) and that appeased more as a patchwork of checks, and (3) presented in adjacent, nonoverlapping areas. Our results showed that the McCollough effect was significantly greater in the valid transparency condition than in the invalid transparency conditions. The effect in the valid transparency condition was nevertheless less strong than was the effect seen with the standard test stimulus made up of nonoverlapping vertical and horizontal stripes, Our results suggest that the McCollough effect can be mediated by the subjective spatial organization (inner representation of vertical and horizontal stripes) that accompanies the perception of transparency in our stimulus. 相似文献
216.
Juan Botella Maria L. Garcia Maribel Barriopedro 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1992,52(5):547-552
A rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) paradigm using both one and two response dimensions was used to test parallel processing models of stimulus dimensions. Fifty subjects were asked to report the identity and/or color of a target uppercase word inserted in a series of lowercase words. The results produced a predominance of posttarget intrusions for color responses and a predominance of pretarget intrusions for identity responses. The requirement of a response to a second dimension impaired hit rates but did not change the pattern of intrusions. An examination of the distributions of intrusions in each response dimension as a function of the response given to the other dimension showed an unexpectedly high percentage of simultaneous hits, a moderate covariation between both responses, and the same patterns of intrusions when compared with the general distributions. While these results seem to be compatible with parallel models of processing for stimulus dimensions, two modifications to this model are suggested. First, the processing of response dimension(s) needs some attentional resources. Second, provision for a mixed model is indicated, which would include trials where no illusory conjunctions are formed. 相似文献
217.
Jonathan Grainger J. Kevin O’Regan Arthur M. Jacobs Juan Segui 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1992,51(1):49-56
Two experiments are described that measured lexical decision latencies and errors to five-letter French words with a single higher frequency orthographic neighbor and control words with no higher frequency neighbors. The higher frequency neighbor differed from the stimulus word by either the second letter (e.g.,astre-autre) or the fourth letter (chope-chose). Neighborhood frequency effects were found to interact with this factor, and significant interference was observed only tochope-type words. The effects of neighborhood frequency were also found to interact with the position of initial fixation in the stimulus word (either the second letter or the fourth letter). Interference was greatly reduced when the initial fixation was on the critical disambiguating letter (i.e., the letterp inchope). Moreover, word recognition was improved when subjects initially fixated the second letter relative to when they initially fixated the fourth letter of a five-letter word, but this second-letter advantage practically disappeared when the stimulus differed from a more frequent word by its fourth letter. The results are interpreted in terms of the interaction between visual and lexical factors in visual word recognition. 相似文献
218.
Millsap and Meredith (1988) have developed a generalization of principal components analysis for the simultaneous analysis of a number of variables observed in several populations or on several occasions. The algorithm they provide has some disadvantages. The present paper offers two alternating least squares algorithms for their method, suitable for small and large data sets, respectively. Lower and upper bounds are given for the loss function to be minimized in the Millsap and Meredith method. These can serve to indicate whether or not a global optimum for the simultaneous components analysis problem has been attained.Financial support by the Netherlands organization for scientific research (NWO) is gratefully acknowledged. 相似文献
219.
Microinjections of leupeptin in the frontal cortex or dorsal hippocampus block spatial learning in the rat 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Adult male Long-Evans hooded rats were given bilateral microinjections of 18 mM leupeptin or saline through cannulae implanted with tips aimed at the frontal cortex or CA1 or CA3 hippocampal cell fields. Five minutes following injections animals were allowed to complete an eight-arm radial maze. The acquisition criterion required that the animal make 7 correct choices of the first 8, and 8 correct choices of the first 10, for five consecutive sessions. Leupeptin slowed acquisition of the eight-arm radial maze task when injected into the CA1 and CA3 hippocampal fields or the frontal cortex, but did not influence spontaneous activity. These results suggest that earlier reports of the amnestic effect of leupeptin when administered into the lateral cerebral ventricle may have been due to effects within the cortex and hippocampus. The present experiment represents the first attempt to identify behaviorally those brain areas in which leupeptin acts to alter learning. 相似文献
220.
Context effects in stroop-like word and picture processing 总被引:7,自引:0,他引:7
Presents a series of 6 experiments in which Stroop-like effects were generated by modally pure color-color, picture-picture, and word-word stimuli instead of the usual modally mixed color-word or picture-word stimuli. Naming, reading, and categorization tasks were applied. The Stroop inhibition was preserved with these stimuli but unexpectedly showed a semantic gradient only in the naming and not in the reading task. Word categorizing was slower and more interference prone than picture categorizing. These and other results can be captured by a model with two main assumptions: (a) semantic memory and the lexicon are separate, and (b) words have privileged access to the lexicon, whereas pictures and colors have privileged access to the semantic network. Such a model is developed and put to an initial test. 相似文献