全文获取类型
收费全文 | 108738篇 |
免费 | 2072篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
110815篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 1174篇 |
2019年 | 1434篇 |
2018年 | 1978篇 |
2017年 | 2042篇 |
2016年 | 2157篇 |
2015年 | 1551篇 |
2014年 | 1823篇 |
2013年 | 8613篇 |
2012年 | 3384篇 |
2011年 | 3519篇 |
2010年 | 2196篇 |
2009年 | 2203篇 |
2008年 | 3052篇 |
2007年 | 3064篇 |
2006年 | 2797篇 |
2005年 | 2441篇 |
2004年 | 2353篇 |
2003年 | 2231篇 |
2002年 | 2264篇 |
2001年 | 3375篇 |
2000年 | 3221篇 |
1999年 | 2437篇 |
1998年 | 1216篇 |
1997年 | 1084篇 |
1996年 | 1118篇 |
1995年 | 1010篇 |
1993年 | 1005篇 |
1992年 | 2042篇 |
1991年 | 1925篇 |
1990年 | 1917篇 |
1989年 | 1767篇 |
1988年 | 1736篇 |
1987年 | 1653篇 |
1986年 | 1675篇 |
1985年 | 1743篇 |
1984年 | 1487篇 |
1983年 | 1322篇 |
1982年 | 994篇 |
1979年 | 1569篇 |
1978年 | 1160篇 |
1975年 | 1273篇 |
1974年 | 1407篇 |
1973年 | 1511篇 |
1972年 | 1263篇 |
1971年 | 1189篇 |
1970年 | 1051篇 |
1969年 | 1067篇 |
1968年 | 1340篇 |
1967年 | 1213篇 |
1966年 | 1090篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
821.
The electromyogram (EMG) recorded with surface electrodes was examined using a triggered oscilloscope technique. When all but the high-amplitude waveforms in a given segment of activity were excluded from analysis, it was found that the duration of the remaining waveforms was specific to the muscle that produced them. That is, waveforms of a given duration were recorded whenever conventional indices-observable contraction, joint movement. needle recordings-showed activity in a specific muscle. This led to the suggestion that waveform duration might be used to validate the surface EMG. The suggestion. which is limited by the need to control posture and electrode placement, both of which may influence waveform duration, was illustrated in application to psychological research. 相似文献
822.
L. J. Herberg J. E. Blundell 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1970,22(2):133-141
Lesions of the hypothalamic ventromedial nucleus (VMN) led to an immediate increase in food intake but not to any increase in hoarding activity. Later, when subjects were very obese, they failed to hoard even in response to a 16-hr. deprivation schedule, although this schedule did produce hoarding if body weights were held at, or were brought down to, preoperative levels. These results indicate that the lateral hypothalamic mechanism responsible for hoarding and feeding responds to long-term nutritional factors, and that it is not directly affected by the short-term satiety mechanisms in the VMN. These findings support the hypothesis (a) that even “non-physiological” activities (e.g. hoarding, exploration) are motivated by physiological needs, and (b) that the reason these activities do not ordinarily covary with physiological drives is that, unlike the physiological drives, they are not subject to inhibition by the hypothalamic satiety mechanisms. 相似文献
823.
Two groups of eight Ss each and one group of seven Ss were exposed to optical tilts (T) of 50, 40, 30, 20, and 10 deg in succession. Exposure time at each tilt was 3, 15, and 27 min in Groups 1, 2, and 3, respectively. Trend analyses of the functions relating level of adaptation to T showed significant quadratic components for Group 1, quadratic and linear components for Group 2, and only linear components for Group 3. These results were consistent with derivations from a memory-comparator model of perceptual adaptation. 相似文献
824.
Stanley and Furedy (1966), using the paired-comparisons technique, reported that under conditions where size constancy held, Emmert’s law failed as distance increased beyond 90 in. Experiment 1, designed to check this report with a shorter-duration induction flash, did not yield 90 in. as the point of failure. However, although complete size constancy for direct images was again obtained, the results did not unambiguously suggest strict proportionality between afterimage size and distance. To allow a less ambiguous assessment of proportionality, Experiment 2 used magnitude-estimation procedures. Distance estimates were completely veridical, but afterimage size varied only linearly, and not proportionally, with apparent distance. It was concluded the Boring (1940) was mistaken in his attempt to reduce size constancy and Emmert’s law to the same principle. 相似文献
825.
C. G. Miles N. J. Mackintosh R. F. Westbrook 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1970,22(3):478-483
Twenty-eight pigeons were given discrimination training. Four groups were run in which for 0, 2, 4 and 9 sessions of training colour was an irrelevant cue and tone was relevant. Generalization tests were given in extinction; the training colour (C1) and another colour (C2) were presented with tone (T) and noise (N). Responding to these four stimulus combinations, TC1, TC2, NC1 and NC2, allowed the assessment of control by colour and tone. A reduction of control by colour and an increase in control by tone were found to be positively related to the amount of training with colour irrelevant. Tests showed a strong inverse relationship between control by colour and tone. 相似文献
826.
C. M. Holloway 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1970,22(3):467-474
The perceptual tuning hypothesis can be expressed as a prediction that discrimination of a particular stimulus dimension will be enhanced if it is the only dimension requiring discrimination and not one of a set of dimensions in a multidimensional discrimination task. In previous tests of the tuning hypothesis, using speech material, the size of a message set has been manipulated as an indirect means of controlling the number of dimensions an observer is required to discriminate. In the present experiments the particular items appearing in a message set have been selected in order to control precisely the number of dimensions relevant to the discrimination task, and performance is estimated in terms of the discrimination of each relevant dimension. No evidence is found for a perceptual tuning effect even though it is argued that the conditions of the present experiment represent those most favourable for its elicitation. 相似文献
827.
James M. Swanson Delos D. Wickens 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1970,22(3):378-383
The release from PI methodology was used to test Oldfield's (1966) suggestion that human memory is organized in part on the basis of frequency of occurrence. An observed release from PI identified frequency of occurrence as an encoding category and provided support for the underlying assumptions of Oldfield's model. 相似文献
828.
M. B. Sterman J. J. Lynch M. T. Orne D. Paskewitz J. Costello N. Nicolov O. Diankov M. Popova E. Tsvetanska Robert G. Grenell J. J. Izquierdo V. H. Mark Joseph Germana N. Zill William P. Paré Joseph E. O. Newton George C. Offutt Walter Ehrlich Joseph E. D. Newton Julij Tosef J. Perez-Cruet Chester R. Wilpizeski John F. Lontz Andrew Livingston Joseph W. Cullen Samuel A. Corson Herman R. Weed Elizabeth O. Corson O. D. Murphree Paul N. Morgan Ruth Jarman J. Antal T. A. Ban J. V. Ananth H. E. Lehmann A. Ulric Moore Richard H. Barnes Wilson G. Pond 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1970,5(3):185-193
829.
830.
M. J. Morgan 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1970,22(4):663-673
In a fixed interval schedule of reinforcement the only responses to be reinforced are those made when a certain time interval has elapsed since the previous reinforcement. The behaviour of three rats on such a schedule was compared with their behaviour on a schedule where a response made at any time during the interval was reinforced by setting up a reward which was delivered when the interval had elapsed. Response rates were higher in the ordinary fixed interval schedule than in its modified version, and it is argued that this rules out attempts to explain the maintenance of fixed interval performance by delayed reinforcement. Despite the clear difference in response rates, there was considerable similarity between the post-reinforcement pauses developed in the two schedules, and this suggests that pausing is influenced more by temporal than by response contingencies. 相似文献