首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2449篇
  免费   77篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2021年   29篇
  2020年   27篇
  2019年   35篇
  2018年   44篇
  2017年   41篇
  2016年   52篇
  2015年   34篇
  2014年   38篇
  2013年   259篇
  2012年   69篇
  2011年   117篇
  2010年   53篇
  2009年   50篇
  2008年   82篇
  2007年   81篇
  2006年   71篇
  2005年   67篇
  2004年   86篇
  2003年   65篇
  2002年   76篇
  2001年   54篇
  2000年   40篇
  1999年   29篇
  1998年   32篇
  1997年   32篇
  1996年   31篇
  1995年   37篇
  1994年   31篇
  1993年   28篇
  1992年   35篇
  1991年   28篇
  1990年   29篇
  1989年   32篇
  1988年   28篇
  1987年   26篇
  1986年   24篇
  1985年   31篇
  1983年   31篇
  1982年   23篇
  1981年   25篇
  1980年   26篇
  1979年   29篇
  1978年   32篇
  1977年   30篇
  1976年   32篇
  1975年   31篇
  1974年   27篇
  1973年   34篇
  1968年   23篇
  1966年   22篇
排序方式: 共有2527条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
971.
Why is our system, which is so biased toward the provision of acute care, underserving the chronically ill?...I think the main reason...has to do with the difficulty of providing for the needs of the chronically ill through a system of largely employment-based private insurance. People who are chronically ill are likely to drop out of employment: they are not a group that employment-based insurance is intended to serve or that employers have an interest in serving....I do not believe we can shift the focus of organization of services for the chronically ill to home or family, as Douard suggests, without reforming the insurance system....  相似文献   
972.
The present study firstly considers the effectiveness of two invite methods. Three hundred and twenty-five patients aged between 30 and 50 were invited to attend a general health check either by letter or opportunistically during a routine consultation After six months the two invite methods had similar attendance rates, although the opportunistic method produced fewer patients at screening and was biased in favour of females. The study also examined the health beliefs of attenders (n = 98) and non-attenders (n = 33) who had been sent questionnaires based on the health belief model prior to the commencement of the screening programme. Discriminant analysis revealed attenders to be more likely to report cutting back on everyday activities when ill and to believe in the seriousness of high blood pressure and weight problems. Non-attenders were found to be more worried about the screening appointment. These findings have important implications for those who plan screening services.  相似文献   
973.
A series of four experiments was designed to investigate the minimal amounts of information required to perceive the structure of a smoothly curved surface from its pattern of projected motion. In Experiments 1 and 2, observers estimated the amplitudes of sinusoidally corrugated surfaces relative to their periods. Observers’ judgments varied linearly with the depicted surface amplitudes, but the amount of perceived relative depth was systematically overestimated by approximately 30%. The observers’ amplitude judgments were also influenced to a lesser extent by the amount of rotary displacement of a surface at each frame transition, and by increasing the length of the apparent motion sequences from two to eight frames. The latter effect of sequence length was quite small, however, accounting for less than 3% of the variance in the observers’ judgments. Experiments 3 and 4 examined observers’ discrimination thresholds for sinusoidally corrugated surfaces of variable amplitude and for ellipsoid surfaces of variable eccentricity. The results revealed that observers could reliably detect differences of surface structure as small as 5%. The length of the apparent motion sequences had no detectable effect on these tasks, although there were significant effects of angular displacement and surface orientation. These results are considered with respect to the analysis of affine structure from motion proposed by Todd and Bressan (1990).  相似文献   
974.
Role of specific similarity in a medical diagnostic task.   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
Three experiments are reported showing that diagnosis of skin disorders by medical residents and general practitioners was facilitated by similar cases previously seen in the same context. Diagnosis of similar cases was facilitated more than that of dissimilar cases in the same diagnostic category, demonstrating that facilitation was not solely due to activation of the diagnostic category as a whole. Because diagnosis was posed in a multiple-choice format that always included the correct diagnosis, the relative disadvantage of dissimilar items was not due to the unavailability of the category name. The similarity effect also occurred with 2-week delay between the initial case and the test cases. Variations in diagnostic procedure, ranging from giving a quick first impression to arguing for given alternative diagnoses before selection, did not interact with the effect of similarity. This result suggests that the similarity effect is not strongly dependent on a particular diagnostic strategy.  相似文献   
975.
Ekman and Friesen (1986) claimed to have discovered a facial expression, a unilateral lip curl, universally recognized as conveying contempt. Their conclusion was based on a series of labeling studies, all of which relied on one response measure—subjects choosing one label from a small, preselected list. This article reports two studies on the question of whether their result can be replicated with other response measures. In one study, subjects were allowed to respond with any emotion label they wanted; in the second, subjects were asked to make quantitative ratings on six emotion scales. Neither method suggested contempt as subjects' interpretation of the unilateral lip curl.This study was funded by a grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. I thank Dee-Ann Matsugu, Lara Weick, and Lisa Wong for their careful work on this study.  相似文献   
976.
The study investigated the role of cognitive complexity as a moderator of the similarity-attraction relationship described by Byrne (1971). Subjects conducted face-to-face interviews with confederates who played roles as job applicants. Similarity-dissimilarity was manipulated by the confederate roles, as well as by the information given the subjects in accordance with the procedures described by Byrne (1971). The situation was viewed as one in which judgments were made about a complex, multidimensional stimulus. Information received about another person in an interview setting was viewed as consisting of different dimensions to be assessed by the perceiver. The hypothesis was tested and confirmed that cognitively complex judges were more likely than simple judges to perceive and evaluate similarity/dissimilarity in others. Thus, complex judges evaluated similar applicants more positively than dissimilar applicants, while no differences were found for simple judges. Cognitive complexity thus appears to moderate the “Law of Attraction” described by Byrne (1971).  相似文献   
977.
978.
The efficacy of several methods of aversive control of excessive alcoholic drinking was investigated in a semi-naturalistic setting that permitted objective measurement of the drinking behavior of chronic alcoholics. Studies 1A and 1B compared an escape-conditioning procedure with a control procedure in which aversive electrical shocks were administered before drinking. Neither procedure effectively decreased subjects' pretreatment, baseline alcoholic drinking behavior. In Study 2, aversive response-contingent shocks effectively suppressed alcoholic drinking, but drinking subsequently returned to its former levels after withdrawal of punishment. Self-administered shock appeared to be as effective as experimenter-administered punishment for controlling drinking, even when the punishment contingency was faded out over time. Study 3 replicated the suppressant effect of punishment, and demonstrated that contingent shock was significantly more effective than yoked, noncontingent shock. A direct comparison of self- versus experimenter-administered punishment suggested a possible slight advantage for the latter.  相似文献   
979.
With both primary and early secondary prevention in mind, this investigation sought to answer several basic questions regarding the resolution of traumas and their conversion into strens - growth-potentiating experiences. College volunteers (N = 40) described in detail their significant positive and negative experiences. The results indicate that many people did experience a trauma that was converted into a stren, and the resolution appeared to be primarily a cognitive process, usually done by the subject himself, and it most frequently occurred between 2 weeks and 4 months after the precipitating event.  相似文献   
980.
The results of two experiments demonstrated that retention deficits occurred in a within-subjects design only when the intermediate interval test was either the first or second test of the retention of prior aversive conditioning experience; when two relearning tests preceded a 1-hr test, no performance deficits were observed. It was suggested that internal cues may serve as memory attributes of avoidance behavior during early stages of avoidance learning, while other stimuli may facilitate the retrieval of the memory of prior avoidance training with repeated testing.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号