首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   6597篇
  免费   282篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2023年   43篇
  2022年   29篇
  2021年   50篇
  2020年   121篇
  2019年   120篇
  2018年   164篇
  2017年   157篇
  2016年   216篇
  2015年   152篇
  2014年   131篇
  2013年   711篇
  2012年   275篇
  2011年   350篇
  2010年   208篇
  2009年   238篇
  2008年   274篇
  2007年   317篇
  2006年   258篇
  2005年   247篇
  2004年   259篇
  2003年   256篇
  2002年   248篇
  2001年   135篇
  2000年   115篇
  1999年   110篇
  1998年   113篇
  1997年   94篇
  1996年   90篇
  1995年   90篇
  1994年   95篇
  1993年   69篇
  1992年   84篇
  1991年   55篇
  1990年   61篇
  1989年   49篇
  1988年   30篇
  1987年   45篇
  1986年   48篇
  1985年   54篇
  1984年   38篇
  1983年   59篇
  1982年   54篇
  1981年   43篇
  1980年   41篇
  1979年   50篇
  1978年   62篇
  1977年   43篇
  1976年   47篇
  1975年   29篇
  1973年   35篇
排序方式: 共有6880条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
102.
103.
104.
The strict competence hypothesis has sparked a small dialogue among several researchers attempting to understand its ramifications for human sentence processing and incremental interpretation in particular. In this paper, we review the dialogue, reconstructing the arguments in an attempt to make them more uniform and crisp, and provide our own analyses of certain of the issues that arise. We argue that strict competence, because it requires a synchronous computation mechanism, may actually lead to more complex, rather than simpler, models of incremental interpretation. Asynchronous computation, which is arguably both psychologically more plausible and conceptually more basic, allows for incremental interpretation to fall out naturally, without additional machinery for interpreting partial constituents. We show that this is true regardless of whether the presumed interpretation mechanism is top-down or bottom-up, contra previous conclusions in the literature, and propose a particular implementation of some of these ideas using a novel representation based on tree-adjoining grammars.The research in this paper was supported in part by grant IRI-9157996 from the National Science Foundation to the first author. The authors would like to thank Fernando Pereira, Edward Stabler, and Mark Steedman for discussions on the topic of this paper and for their comments on previous drafts.  相似文献   
105.
A study is reported that tests the hypothesis that group members exhibit intergroup bias in response to the belief that outsiders will discriminate against them. To this end, two experimental conditions are included in which subjects anticipate either biased evaluations or fair evaluations respectively. In a control condition, subjects do not expect to be evaluated from an external source. Results indicated, as expected, that those who anticipated biased evaluations from an outgroup exhibited bias themselves, while those who anticipated fair evaluations exhibited outgroup favouritism. The fact that control subjects exhibited the same degree of bias as those who anticipated biased evaluations from the outgroup poses some difficulties for the hypothesized connection between anticipated discrimination and intergoup bias. Thus, it appears that intergroup bias is the rule and not the exception in an intergroup context. Nevertheless, it is clear that anticipated evaluations of outgroup members can effect intergroup bias.  相似文献   
106.
107.
108.
A Final Comment on the Case of the Family Environment Scale   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   
109.
A model is presented for item responses when different subjects employ different strategies, but only responses, not choice of strategy, can be observed. Using substantive theory to differentiate the likelihoods of response vectors under a fixed set of strategies, we model response probabilities in terms of item parameters for each strategy, proportions of subjects employing each strategy, and distributions of subject proficiency within strategies. The probabilities that an individual subject employed the various strategies can then be obtained, along with a conditional estimate of proficiency under each. A conceptual example discusses response strategies for spatial rotation tasks, and a numerican example resolves a population of subjects into subpopulations of valid responders and random guessers.The first author's work was supported by Contract No. N00014-85-K-0683, project designation NR 150-539, from the Cognitive Science Program, Cognitive and Neural Sciences Division, Office of Naval Research. We are grateful to Murray Aitkin, Isaac Bejar, Neil Dorans, Frederiksen, and Marklyn Wingersky for their comments and suggestions, and to Alison Gooding, Maxine Kingston, Donna Lembeck, Joling Liang, and Kentaro Yamamoto for their assistance with Example 2.  相似文献   
110.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号