全文获取类型
收费全文 | 287篇 |
免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
290篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 1篇 |
2021年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 3篇 |
2018年 | 2篇 |
2017年 | 1篇 |
2016年 | 7篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 10篇 |
2013年 | 36篇 |
2012年 | 19篇 |
2011年 | 12篇 |
2010年 | 6篇 |
2009年 | 17篇 |
2008年 | 13篇 |
2007年 | 19篇 |
2006年 | 9篇 |
2005年 | 8篇 |
2004年 | 6篇 |
2003年 | 8篇 |
2002年 | 10篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 7篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有290条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
Lars Lewejohann Anne Marie Hoppmann Philipp Kegel Mareike Kritzler Antonio Krüger Norbert Sachser 《Behavior research methods》2009,41(3):850-856
Neurodegen erative disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD) are increasingly threatening public health. Most animal models
of AD consist of transgenic mice that are usually housed singly or in unisexual groups in small barren cages. Such restricted
environments, however, prevent the mice from showing a variety of speciesspecific behaviors and consequently may constrain
comprehensive behavioral phenotyping. On the other hand, allowing the animals to freely organize their lives in a spacious
physically and socially enriched environment makes behavioral phenotyping laborious and time consuming. Radio frequency identification
(RFID) using a network of antennae and small glass-coated transponders labeling each individual allows for gathering spatiotemporal
information about a large number of individuals in parallel. The aim of this project was to use the RFID technique to facilitate
the characterization of mice carrying a genetic disposition to develop AD-like pathology and of their wild-type conspecifics
in a spacious seminaturalistic environment. 相似文献
92.
93.
In two experiments, we tested accessibility experiences versus accessible content in influencing the hindsight bias when participants generated either thoughts about alternative outcomes or thoughts about known outcomes. Participants who had listed many thoughts (Experiment 1) and those who had contracted their brow muscles (Experiment 2) when considering alternate outcomes rated the known outcome as more likely than did than those who had listed two thoughts or who had not contracted their brows--a "backfire" effect. In contrast, but no less ironically, participants who had listed many thoughts and those who contracted their brows when considering known outcomes rated those outcomes as less likely--an "it could never have happened" effect. Both effects are due to subjective accessibility experiences, and their role in influencing and debiasing the hindsight bias is discussed. 相似文献
94.
Norbert Jaušovec 《创造力研究杂志》2013,25(4):349-366
The aim of the study was to investigate the relationship between flexible strategic thinking and problem solving performance. In the first experiment, gifted, average, and poor problem solvers performed a number of tasks which were designed to provoke rigidity. The results indicated that the average and poor performance groups did not differ in the number of answers indicating response and perceptual set. On the other hand, creative and intelligent students showed fewer rigid answers than the other two groups. In the second experiment, gifted and average students were asked to think aloud while solving different problems with open and closed solution situations. The thinking aloud protocols were analyzed by classifying the statements into different strategy types. Gifted students used a variety of strategies when solving problems, as well as different strategies for different problem types. No such differences were observed with the average problem solvers. The results of both experiments demonstrated consistent evidence for the role of flexible strategic thinking in gifted problem solving, as well as less direct evidence of the importance of metacognitive knowledge. 相似文献
95.
Alexander J. Rothman Geoffrey Haddock Norbert Schwarz 《Journal of applied social psychology》2001,31(10):2195-2214
The effectiveness of interventions that directly inform people of the risks posed by their own behavior is frequently undermined by people's ability to defend themselves against unwanted information. In response to this difficulty, an alternative approach to shaping people's perceptions of personal vulnerability in which people remained unaware of the intervention was tested. As part of a survey on sexual issues, college undergraduates reported the total number of sexual partners they had had on a response scale that systematically framed their sexual behavior as being either above average or below average. Participants in the 2 conditions reported the same absolute number of sexual partners. However, those who received a response scale that implied they had had more partners than the average student reported greater concern about their sexual behavior than did those who received a scale that implied they had had fewer partners than average. The implications of this approach for influencing perceptions of personal risk are discussed. 相似文献
96.
Robert F. Belli Norbert Schwarz Eleanor Singer Jennifer Talarico 《Applied cognitive psychology》2000,14(4):295-308
In survey research, the use of decomposition can lead to pronounced reporting errors as seen by overreporting and overall reporting error. A total of 87 subjects answered either decomposed or undecomposed questions concerning telephone calls made by them while at work. The questionnaire conditions varied the length of the reference period (1 week or 6 months), and the type of call (local or long‐distance). Decomposition conditions introduced either spatial or temporal cues. In all comparisons, decomposed questions increased overreporting bias relative to undecomposed questions. In addition, undecomposed questions with a 1‐week reference period led to increased overreporting bias in comparison to undecomposed/6‐month questions. Results are consistent with a category split estimation model in which smaller categories are predicted to lead to overreporting, and larger categories to underreporting. Decomposition is not recommended for gaining retrospective reports of non‐distinctive, frequent events. Copyright © 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
97.
98.
Norbert Schwarz 《Journal of Consumer Psychology》2006,16(4):324-324
Psychologists have long portrayed the mind “as a machine or computer that is the same in all times and places, while only the raw materials processed by the machinery or the data in the computer vary” (Fiske, Kitayama, Markus, & Nisbett, 1998, p. 918). This state of affairs changed dramatically over the last decade and a rapidly growing body of experimental research documents profound cultural differences in basic psychological processes. To date, much of this work has addressed the influence of individualism or collectivism, often framed in terms of independent vs. interdependent self‐construals (Markus & Kitayama, 1991; for a comprehensive review, see Oyserman, Coon, & Kemmelmeier, 2002). Other dimensions of culture, which figured prominently in Hofstede's (1980) groundbreaking analysis, received less attention. In this issue's Research Dialogue, Shavitt, Lalwani, Zhang, and Torelli (2006) draw attention to one of these dimensions, namely the distinction between horizontal and vertical cultural orientations, which value equality or emphasize hierarchy, respectively. They review the importance of this distinction for understanding personal values, advertising and consumer persuasion, self‐presentational patterns, and gender differences, noting that the influence of horizontal/vertical orientations is often obscured by their conflation with differences in individualism and collectivism. Commentaries by Aaker (2006), Meyers‐Levy (2006), and Oyserman (2006) elaborate on their analysis and suggest additional avenues for future research. In combination, this set of papers makes a compelling case for extending the analysis of cultural differences in cognition, emotion, motivation, and behavior beyond the individualism/collectivism distinction that has captured the lion's share of recent attention. 相似文献
99.
100.
Based on a conversational analysis of experimental procedures and consistent with the principle of relevance, we predicted that participants' verbal responses will be influenced by their tacit inferences about the researcher's epistemic goals, derived from their knowledge of the researcher's academic affiliation. We tested this prediction in a core area of social‐personality and cultural psychology, causal attribution. University students provided causal attributions about mass murder cases, while the questionnaire identified the researcher either as a social scientist or a personality psychologist. The results indicated that attributions were overall more situational than dispositional, and as predicted, this main effect was qualified by an interaction between conversational cue and type of attribution. Thus, participants gave relatively more situational explanations when the letterhead of the questionnaire identified the researcher as a social scientist compared to when the researcher was identified as a personality psychologist. The reverse pattern emerged for dispositional attributions. Methodological and conceptual implications are discussed. Copyright © 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献