首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   320篇
  免费   16篇
  336篇
  2020年   9篇
  2019年   7篇
  2018年   11篇
  2017年   14篇
  2016年   10篇
  2015年   6篇
  2014年   7篇
  2013年   34篇
  2012年   10篇
  2011年   14篇
  2010年   9篇
  2009年   11篇
  2008年   9篇
  2007年   13篇
  2006年   10篇
  2005年   8篇
  2004年   7篇
  2003年   10篇
  2002年   7篇
  2001年   12篇
  2000年   10篇
  1999年   2篇
  1998年   4篇
  1997年   3篇
  1994年   3篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   9篇
  1991年   4篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   6篇
  1984年   5篇
  1983年   4篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   5篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   2篇
  1978年   3篇
  1976年   2篇
  1974年   2篇
  1973年   6篇
  1971年   8篇
  1970年   2篇
  1966年   4篇
  1959年   1篇
  1956年   2篇
  1954年   1篇
  1950年   1篇
  1940年   1篇
排序方式: 共有336条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
121.
The purpose of the current study was to determine relationships between orthographic and morphological awareness of academic words and achievement across content areas. Participants (n = 256), diverse seventh and eighth graders, took three word knowledge measures; two standardized achievement measures were used as outcomes. Orthographic awareness of academic words explained variance in achievement, beyond overall breadth of vocabulary knowledge, with significant changes in R2 ranging from 0.02 to 0.08. Morphological awareness of academic words explained variance in achievement, beyond overall breadth of vocabulary knowledge, with significant changes in R2 ranging from 0.06 to 0.16.  相似文献   
122.
Abstract

We examined longitudinal changes in young adolescents’ (N=368) action-control beliefs, coping behaviors, and adjustment (i.e., positive and negative affect, depression, aggression) across the transition from elementary school to middle school. Results indicated greater inter-individual instability in adjustment during this transition than during the previous school year. Using ordinary least-squares (OLS) growth models to extract intra-individual change scores for each variable (i.e., slopes and intercepts), we conducted a series of stepwise regressions to determine which features of control beliefs and coping behaviors best predicted changes in adjustment across the transition to middle school. We found that negative coping behaviors (i.e., antisocial coping) consistently predicted negative changes in the adjustment variables (e.g., greater depression, more aggression), whereas positive beliefs and behaviors did not consistently predict changes in the adjustment variables.  相似文献   
123.
Measures of rate of change and of rate of rate of change are developed for application to physiological and psychological data. Examples are given of the use of these measures with heart-rate data for comparisons within and between subjects. It is shown that the measures provide information over and above that offered by means and variances, and that the measures appropriately reflect the impact of external variables.  相似文献   
124.
One of the more important constructs in the study of attention, perception, and cognition is that of capacity. The authors reviewed some of the common meanings of this construct and proposed a more precise treatment. They showed how the distribution of response times can be used to derive measures of process capacity and to further illustrate how these measures can be used to address important hypotheses in cognition.  相似文献   
125.
In accordance with the determination of base-line heart rate and running in an activity wheel, 14 Long-Evans male rats were given ten 30 minute sessions of Immobile-Avoid conditioning followed by 30 minute sessions of Active-Avoid conditioning. Control Ss were yoked to the experimental Ss.

Experimental Ss made significantly fewer responses in the immobile-avoid period and significantly more responses in the active-avoid period than did the yoked control Ss. Heart rate was correlated with skeletal activity both for experimental and control Ss. For experimental Ss, low heart rate was concomitant with a low amount of activity in the immobile-avoid period and high heart rate was concomitant with greater skeletal activity in the active-avoid period.

These results support the position that heart rate is secondary to responses of the somatic-motor system and that heart rate during immobile-avoid or active avoid conditioning is more closely related to the level of somatic-motor activity than to the emotional stress which purportedly motivates instrumental avoidance behavior.  相似文献   
126.
Machery  Edouard  Stich  Stephen  Rose  David  Alai  Mario  Angelucci  Adriano  Berniūnas  Renatas  Buchtel  Emma E.  Chatterjee  Amita  Cheon  Hyundeuk  Cho  In-Rae  Cohnitz  Daniel  Cova  Florian  Dranseika  Vilius  Lagos  Ángeles Eraña  Ghadakpour  Laleh  Grinberg  Maurice  Hannikainen  Ivar  Hashimoto  Takaaki  Horowitz  Amir  Hristova  Evgeniya  Jraissati  Yasmina  Kadreva  Veselina  Karasawa  Kaori  Kim  Hackjin  Kim  Yeonjeong  Lee  Minwoo  Mauro  Carlos  Mizumoto  Masaharu  Moruzzi  Sebastiano  Olivola  Christopher Y.  Ornelas  Jorge  Osimani  Barbara  Romero  Carlos  Lopez  Alejandro Rosas  Sangoi  Massimo  Sereni  Andrea  Songhorian  Sarah  Sousa  Paulo  Struchiner  Noel  Tripodi  Vera  Usui  Naoki  del Mercado  Alejandro Vázquez  Volpe  Giorgio  Vosgerichian  Hrag Abraham  Zhang  Xueyi  Zhu  Jing 《印度哲学研究理事会杂志》2017,34(3):517-541
Journal of Indian Council of Philosophical Research - This article examines whether people share the Gettier intuition (viz. that someone who has a true justified belief that p may nonetheless fail...  相似文献   
127.
Young people are spending increasing amounts of time using digital technology and, as such, are at great risk of being involved in cyber bullying as a victim, bully, or bully/victim. Despite cyber bullying typically occurring outside the school environment, the impact of being involved in cyber bullying is likely to spill over to school. Fully 285 11- to 15-year-olds (125 male and 160 female, M age = 12.19 years, SD = 1.03) completed measures of cyber bullying involvement, self-esteem, trust, perceived peer acceptance, and perceptions of the value of learning and the importance of school. For young women, involvement in cyber bullying as a victim, bully, or bully/victim negatively predicted perceptions of learning and school, and perceived peer acceptance mediated this relationship. The results indicated that involvement in cyber bullying negatively predicted perceived peer acceptance which, in turn, positively predicted perceptions of learning and school. For young men, fulfilling the bully/victim role negatively predicted perceptions of learning and school. Consequently, for young women in particular, involvement in cyber bullying spills over to impact perceptions of learning. The findings of the current study highlight how stressors external to the school environment can adversely impact young women’s perceptions of school and also have implications for the development of interventions designed to ameliorate the effects of cyber bullying.  相似文献   
128.
The Survivor Interaction Contrast (SIC) is a powerful tool for assessing the architecture and stopping rule of a model of mental processes (Townsend & Nozawa, 1995). Despite its demonstrated utility, the methodology has lacked a method for statistical testing until now. In this paper we briefly describe the SIC then develop some basic statistical properties of the measure. These developments lead to a statistical test for rejecting certain classes of models based on the SIC. We verify these tests using simulated data, then demonstrate their use on data from a simple cognitive task.  相似文献   
129.
130.
A defining characteristic of age-related cognitive decline is a deficit in general cognitive performance. Here we use a testing and analysis regimen that allows us to characterize the general learning abilities of young (3-5 mo old) and aged (19-21 mo old) male and female Balb/C mice. Animals' performance was assessed on a battery of seven diverse learning tasks. Aged animals exhibited deficits in five of the seven tasks and ranked significantly lower than their young counterparts in general learning abilities (aggregate performance across the battery of tasks). Aging added variability to common core performance (i.e., general learning ability), which translated into increased variability on the individual cognitive tasks. Relatedly, general learning abilities did not differ between the two ages among the best quartile of learners (i.e., cognitive abilities were spared in a subsample of the aged animals). Additionally, working memory capacity (resistance to interference) and duration (resistance to decay) accounted for significantly more of the variability in general learning abilities in aged relative to young animals. Tests of 15 noncognitive performance variables indicated that an increase in body weight (and an associated decrease in general activity) was characteristic of those aged animals which exhibited deficient general learning abilities. These results suggest the possibility that general cognitive deficits in aged animals reflect a failure of specific components of the working memory system, and may be related to variations in body weight and an associated decrease in activity.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号