全文获取类型
收费全文 | 445篇 |
免费 | 20篇 |
专业分类
465篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 11篇 |
2019年 | 13篇 |
2018年 | 15篇 |
2017年 | 14篇 |
2016年 | 12篇 |
2015年 | 18篇 |
2014年 | 10篇 |
2013年 | 51篇 |
2012年 | 21篇 |
2011年 | 16篇 |
2010年 | 21篇 |
2009年 | 12篇 |
2008年 | 25篇 |
2007年 | 14篇 |
2006年 | 12篇 |
2005年 | 15篇 |
2004年 | 14篇 |
2003年 | 5篇 |
2002年 | 6篇 |
2001年 | 6篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 5篇 |
1970年 | 4篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1943年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有465条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
81.
What Triggers Anger in Everyday Life? Links to the Intensity,Control, and Regulation of These Emotions,and Personality Traits 下载免费PDF全文
Todd B. Kashdan Fallon R. Goodman Travis T. Mallard C. Nathan DeWall 《Journal of personality》2016,84(6):737-749
Why do people experience anger? Most of our knowledge on anger‐triggering events is based on the study of reactions at a single time point in a person's life. Little research has examined how people experience anger in their daily life over time. In this study, we conducted a comprehensive examination of the situational determinants of anger over the course of 3 weeks. Using daily diary methodology, people (N = 173; 2,342 anger episodes) reported their most intense daily anger and, with an open‐ended format, described the trigger. Participants also answered questions on anger intensity, control, and regulatory strategies, along with baseline personality trait measures. Using an iterative coding system, five anger trigger categories emerged: other people, psychological and physical distress, intrapersonal demands, environment, and diffuse/undifferentiated/unknown. Compared with other triggers, when anger was provoked by other people or when the source was unknown, there was a stronger positive association with anger intensity and lack of control. Personality traits (i.e., anger, mindfulness, psychological need satisfaction, the Big Five) showed few links to the experience and regulation of daily anger. Although aversive events often spur anger, the correlates and consequences of anger differ depending on the source of aversion; personality traits offer minimal value in predicting anger in daily life. 相似文献
82.
83.
84.
Craig J. Bryan Tanya Goodman Wayne Chappelle Lillian Prince William Thompson 《Military psychology》2018,30(5):437-448
US Air Force remote warrior personnel include a range of military personnel composed of remotely piloted aircraft aircrew, intelligence operators, and cyber warfare operators. Job duties in these professions entail long work hours, rotating shifts, and regular engagement in combat operations. Although research has identified a number of factors correlated with elevated burnout and emotional distress in this population, little is known about the unique needs of discrete subgroups of distressed personnel. In a sample of 7,550 US Air Force remote warriors, results of latent class analysis yielded four distinct classes. Three of the four reported moderately elevated burnout and job dissatisfaction but differed with respect to demographics and development career stage. The fourth group reported significantly higher levels of emotional distress, burnout, job dissatisfaction, and problems in living. This latter group was comparable to the mid-career group with respect to demographics, although this group had much lower levels of responsibility and indicators of career advancement. Results suggest that, among remote warrior personnel, there are several subtypes of elevated emotional distress. Prevention and intervention strategies that are matched to each subtype may yield better occupational and mental health outcomes than universal, “one size fits all” strategies. 相似文献
85.
86.
Falk Lieder Thomas L. Griffiths Quentin J. M. Huys Noah D. Goodman 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2018,25(1):322-349
Cognitive biases, such as the anchoring bias, pose a serious challenge to rational accounts of human cognition. We investigate whether rational theories can meet this challenge by taking into account the mind’s bounded cognitive resources. We asked what reasoning under uncertainty would look like if people made rational use of their finite time and limited cognitive resources. To answer this question, we applied a mathematical theory of bounded rationality to the problem of numerical estimation. Our analysis led to a rational process model that can be interpreted in terms of anchoring-and-adjustment. This model provided a unifying explanation for ten anchoring phenomena including the differential effect of accuracy motivation on the bias towards provided versus self-generated anchors. Our results illustrate the potential of resource-rational analysis to provide formal theories that can unify a wide range of empirical results and reconcile the impressive capacities of the human mind with its apparently irrational cognitive biases. 相似文献
87.
An Ecological View of School Counselor Ratios and Student Academic Outcomes: A National Investigation 下载免费PDF全文
Emily Goodman‐Scott Christopher A. Sink Blaire E. Cholewa Melanie Burgess 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》2018,96(4):388-398
The effect of student‐to‐school‐counselor ratios on academic outcomes has been widely studied, yet few researchers have taken a multilevel approach. Using the nationally representative High School Longitudinal Study: 2009 dataset, the authors found ratios were significantly associated with student grade point average (GPA) and graduation. Attending a Title I school also was associated with students’ GPA, advanced placement/international baccalaureate credits earned, and postsecondary course taking. The authors discuss implications for advocacy and practice, contextualizing their findings within an ecological framework. 相似文献
88.
89.
Sarah J. Goodman Danielle S. Roubinov Nicole R. Bush Mina Park Pau Farr Eldon Emberly Clyde Hertzman Marilyn J. Essex Michael S. Kobor W. Thomas Boyce 《Developmental science》2019,22(2)
A growing body of research has documented associations between adverse childhood environments and DNA methylation, highlighting epigenetic processes as potential mechanisms through which early external contexts influence health across the life course. The present study tested a complementary hypothesis: indicators of children's early internal, biological, and behavioral responses to stressful challenges may also be linked to stable patterns of DNA methylation later in life. Children's autonomic nervous system reactivity, temperament, and mental health symptoms were prospectively assessed from infancy through early childhood, and principal components analysis (PCA) was applied to derive composites of biological and behavioral reactivity. Buccal epithelial cells were collected from participants at 15 and 18 years of age. Findings revealed an association between early life biobehavioral inhibition/disinhibition and DNA methylation across many genes. Notably, reactive, inhibited children were found to have decreased DNA methylation of the DLX5 and IGF2 genes at both time points, as compared to non‐reactive, disinhibited children. Results of the present study are provisional but suggest that the gene's profile of DNA methylation may constitute a biomarker of normative or potentially pathological differences in reactivity. Overall, findings provide a foundation for future research to explore relations among epigenetic processes and differences in both individual‐level biobehavioral risk and qualities of the early, external childhood environment. 相似文献
90.
Sue Finch Geoff Cumming Jennifer Williams Lee Palmer Elvira Griffith Chris Alders James Anderson Olivia Goodman 《Behavior research methods, instruments & computers》2004,36(2):312-324
Geoffrey Loftus, Editor of Memory & Cognition from 1994 to 1997, strongly encouraged presentation of figures with error bars and avoidance of null hypothesis significance testing (NHST). The authors examined 696 Memory & Cognition articles published before, during, and after the Loftus editorship. Use of figures with bars increased to 47% under Loftus's editorship and then declined. Bars were rarely used for interpretation, and NHST remained almost universal. Analysis of 309 articles in other psychology journals confirmed that Loftus's influence was most evident in the articles he accepted for publication, but was otherwise limited. An e-mail survey of authors of papers accepted by Loftus revealed some support for his policy, but allegiance to traditional practices as well. Reform of psychologists' statistical practices would require more than editorial encouragement. 相似文献