首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   960篇
  免费   63篇
  国内免费   4篇
  2023年   19篇
  2022年   18篇
  2021年   23篇
  2020年   29篇
  2019年   28篇
  2018年   54篇
  2017年   46篇
  2016年   61篇
  2015年   39篇
  2014年   35篇
  2013年   109篇
  2012年   65篇
  2011年   79篇
  2010年   35篇
  2009年   37篇
  2008年   51篇
  2007年   46篇
  2006年   31篇
  2005年   40篇
  2004年   30篇
  2003年   31篇
  2002年   24篇
  2001年   15篇
  2000年   12篇
  1999年   4篇
  1998年   7篇
  1997年   4篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   3篇
  1994年   4篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   4篇
  1991年   3篇
  1990年   2篇
  1989年   2篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   2篇
  1984年   1篇
  1983年   2篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   3篇
  1978年   1篇
  1975年   4篇
  1974年   5篇
  1973年   1篇
  1968年   2篇
  1967年   1篇
  1966年   1篇
  1961年   1篇
  1938年   1篇
排序方式: 共有1027条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
971.
972.
973.
ABSTRACT

Some aspects of experience can be challenging for research participants to verbalise. Interpretative phenomenological analysis (IPA) researchers need to get experience-near to meet their phenomenological commitments, capturing the “texture” and quality of existence, and placing participants in relation to events, objects, others, and the world. Incorporating drawing into IPA designs provides a vehicle through which participants can better explore and communicate their lifeworlds. IPA researchers also require rich accounts to fulfil their interpretative commitments. Drawing taps into multiple sensory registers simultaneously, providing polysemous data, which in turn lends itself to hermeneutic analysis. This article outlines a multimodal method, the relational mapping interview, which was developed to understand the relational context of various forms of distress and disruption. We illustrate how the approach results in richly nuanced visual and verbal accounts of relational experience. Drawing on an “expanded hermeneutic phenomenology,” we suggest how visual data can be analysed within an IPA framework to offer significant experiential insights.  相似文献   
974.
975.
976.
977.
978.
979.
ABSTRACT

Previous work suggests that the estimated age in adults’ earliest autobiographical memories depends on age information implied by the experimental context [e.g., Kingo, O. S., Bohn, A., & Krøjgaard, P. (2013). Warm-up questions on early childhood memories affect the reported age of earliest memories in late adolescence. Memory, 21(2), 280–284. doi:10.1080/09658211.2012.729598] and that the age in decontextualised snippets of memory is younger than in more complete accounts (i.e., event memories [Bruce, D., Wilcox-O’Hearn, L. A., Robinson, J. A., Phillips-Grant, K., Francis, L., & Smith, M. C. (2005). Fragment memories mark the end of childhood amnesia. Memory & Cognition, 33(4), 567–576. doi:10.3758/BF03195324]). We examined the malleability of the estimated age in undergraduates’ earliest memories and its relation with memory quality. In Study 1 (n?=?141), vignettes referring to events happening at age 2 rendered earlier reported ages than examples referring to age 6. Exploratory analyses suggested that event memories were more sensitive to the age manipulation than memories representing a single, isolated scene (i.e., snapshots). In Study 2 (n?=?162), asking self-relevant and public-event knowledge questions about participants’ preschool years prior to retrieval yielded comparable average estimated ages. Both types of semantic knowledge questions rendered earlier memories than a no-age control task. Overall, the reported age in snapshots was younger than in event memories. However, age-differences between memory types across conditions were not statistically significant. Together, the results add to the growing literature indicating that the average age in earliest memories is not as fixed as previously thought.  相似文献   
980.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号