全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3245篇 |
免费 | 37篇 |
国内免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
3290篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 17篇 |
2023年 | 21篇 |
2022年 | 23篇 |
2021年 | 38篇 |
2020年 | 57篇 |
2019年 | 71篇 |
2018年 | 94篇 |
2017年 | 104篇 |
2016年 | 131篇 |
2015年 | 71篇 |
2014年 | 110篇 |
2013年 | 368篇 |
2012年 | 167篇 |
2011年 | 172篇 |
2010年 | 100篇 |
2009年 | 101篇 |
2008年 | 142篇 |
2007年 | 137篇 |
2006年 | 122篇 |
2005年 | 102篇 |
2004年 | 107篇 |
2003年 | 102篇 |
2002年 | 74篇 |
2001年 | 63篇 |
2000年 | 56篇 |
1999年 | 40篇 |
1998年 | 37篇 |
1997年 | 41篇 |
1996年 | 36篇 |
1995年 | 40篇 |
1994年 | 28篇 |
1993年 | 31篇 |
1992年 | 37篇 |
1991年 | 32篇 |
1990年 | 34篇 |
1989年 | 20篇 |
1988年 | 34篇 |
1987年 | 17篇 |
1986年 | 16篇 |
1985年 | 25篇 |
1984年 | 23篇 |
1983年 | 15篇 |
1982年 | 25篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 23篇 |
1978年 | 13篇 |
1977年 | 17篇 |
1974年 | 18篇 |
1972年 | 13篇 |
1968年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有3290条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
In a study among 72 nurses, the affective consequences of social comparison were examined and related to neuroticism (N) and to social comparison orientation (SCO). Participants were confronted with a bogus interview with an upward versus a downward comparison target. Positive affect and identification were higher, and negative affect was lower, in the upward than in the downward comparison condition. Independent of their SCO, the higher individuals were in N, the less they identified with the upward comparison target, the more they identified with the downward comparison target, and the less positive affect they showed following confrontation with the upward comparison target. In contrast, independent of their level of N, the higher individuals were in SCO, the more negative affect they showed following confrontation with the downward comparison target. The effects on negative affect stayed the same when controlling for positive affect, and the effects on positive affect stayed the same when controlling for negative affect. These effects were also obtained when perceived direction was used as a predictor instead of the experimentally manipulated direction. It is concluded that, although N and SCO are correlated, these variables seem to have independent and distinct effects upon the responses to social comparison information. 相似文献
102.
According to the two-thirds power law the cube of the speed of a drawing movement is proportional to the radius of curvature of the trajectory, and the coefficient of proportionality has the meaning of mechanical power. We derive this empirical law from the variational principle known in physics as the principle of least action. It states that if a movement between two points of a given path obeys the two-thirds law, then the amount of work required to execute a trajectory in a fixed time is minimal. In this strict sense one may say that among infinitely many ways to execute a given path, the central nervous system chooses the most economical. We show that the kinematic equations for all drawing movements are solutions of a certain differential equation with a single (time-variable) coefficient. We consider several special cases of drawing movements corresponding to simplest forms of this coefficient. Copyright 2001 Academic Press. 相似文献
103.
Displays were presented consisting of a perspective projection of a regular square grid, made up of vertical and horizontal equally spaced white lines, that was slanted in depth. The surface was viewed monocularly, through a circular aperture. A range of slants was shown (0 degree, 10 degrees, 20 degrees, 30 degrees, 40 degrees, 50 degrees, or 60 degrees) and the observers' task was to match the slant by means of a mouse-driven probe. The viewing distance (50, 75, or 100 cm) as well as the focal distance (25, 50, 75, 100, or 125 cm) were varied. We expected the estimation error to be smallest when the viewing distance and the focal distance coincided. This was not the case. Instead, subjects seemed to use the perspective deformation of the texture elements in the stimulus display to make a slant estimation, regardless of the specific combination of viewing distance and focal distance. 相似文献
104.
Andersen MB Van Raalte JL Brewer BW 《Professional psychology, research and practice》2001,32(1):12-18
Although the parent discipline of sport psychology is psychology, the delivery of sport psychology services has its main roots in physical education and sports science (motor learning and control, skill acquisition). Thus, sport psychologists may look more like coaches than they look like clinicians or counselors. In this article, the authors trace the evolution of sport psychology services and contrast the temporal, spatial, and delivery issues of applied sport psychology with more mainstream counseling and clinical psychology. The looser boundaries of sport psychologist practice have both benefits and dangers, and the authors offer some examples to professional psychologists who are thinking of expanding their delivery of service to athletes and coaches. 相似文献
105.
This work evaluated the prospect that organizational accounts of the retention of list information by monkeys might be an
artifact of familiarity with conditional relationships. Seven sophisticated macaques were trained on four five-item lists.
Each acquisition selectively excluded one of the internal conditional pairs of the typical four-problem sequence (AB,BC,CD,DE)
that defines a five-item serially ordered list. Then, all possible novel pairings and the trained pairs appeared together
in a test. After this, the previously omitted pair was trained and animals were retested. On all tasks, initial tests revealed
little organization and much intersubject variability of characteristic choice strategies, but subsequent inclusion of all
four conditional pairs always yielded organized serial choice. On both the four-problem tests and in a later retention, errors
were directly related to interitem distance between the objects paired on test trials. These results helped to specify the
conditions required for demonstration of non-human primate analogs of transitivity, and showed that even sophisticated monkeys
organize information in retention only if they know all interitem relationships.
Received: 7 October 1998 / Accepted after revision: 10 October 1999 相似文献
106.
107.
108.
Johan Wagemans Luc Van Gool G ry d'Ydewalle 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》1992,44(3):475-508
In previous research on symmetry detection, factors contributing to orientational effects (axis and virtual lines connecting symmetrically positioned dots) and component processes (axis selection and pointwise evaluation) have always been confounded. The reason is the restriction to bilateral symmetry (BS), with pointwise correspondences being orthogonal to the axis of symmetry. In our experiments, subjects had to discriminate random dot patterns from symmetries defined by combining 12 axis orientations (every 15°) with seven reflection angles (0°, yielding BS, and three clockwise and counterclockwise 15° steps, yielding skewed symmetry, SS). In Experiment 1, with completely randomized trial order, a significant interaction between axis and skewing angle was obtained, indicating that classically observed orientational effects are restricted to BS and that the orientation of the pointwise correspondences is important. These basic findings were replicated in three subsequent experiments, which differed in that they used blocks containing patterns with the same axis (Experiment 2), virtual lines orientation (Experiment 3), or their combination (Experiment 4). Based on a comparison between the results obtained by these manipulations, we suggest a possible reason for the failure of preattentive symmetry detection in the case of dot patterns with SS. 相似文献
109.
110.