首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   280篇
  免费   12篇
  2023年   3篇
  2021年   4篇
  2020年   4篇
  2019年   2篇
  2018年   7篇
  2017年   8篇
  2016年   11篇
  2015年   3篇
  2014年   11篇
  2013年   34篇
  2012年   9篇
  2011年   8篇
  2010年   10篇
  2009年   11篇
  2008年   7篇
  2007年   9篇
  2006年   5篇
  2005年   9篇
  2004年   9篇
  2003年   5篇
  2002年   7篇
  2001年   11篇
  2000年   8篇
  1999年   7篇
  1998年   9篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   6篇
  1995年   4篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   2篇
  1990年   3篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   4篇
  1986年   5篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   3篇
  1983年   2篇
  1982年   4篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   1篇
  1974年   4篇
  1973年   3篇
  1972年   2篇
  1971年   2篇
  1970年   1篇
  1969年   1篇
  1968年   1篇
  1967年   1篇
排序方式: 共有292条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
261.
262.
The notion that we read words more easily in story context than in isolation owes much to the influence of a study by Goodman (1965), who found a 60‐80 per cent improvement in reading accuracy when children read words in context rather than in isolation. This study has been widely cited in the reading literature, even though its validity has at times been questioned. In the present .study, Goodman's findings were re‐examined by conducting three experiments. The first involved asking 60 children, all eight years of age, to read stories which varied in difficulty and contextual completeness, and within which target words occurred in varying sentence position. The second experiment, using easy stories, sought to find out whether the results for the first experiment would also be similar for ten good and ten poor readers, also about 8 years of age. The third experiment, involving a further 8 good and 8 poor readers, at the same age level, extended the search for context effects by using difficult stories as well. The results of this series of experiments suggest that Goodman was wrong about the effects of context on word recognition. What seems to separate good readers from poor is the ability to decode words independently of context.  相似文献   
263.
264.
265.
266.
Jolicoeur (1985, Memory & Cognition 13 289-303) found a linear increase in the latency to name line drawings of objects rotated (0 degrees to 120 degrees) from the upright (0 degrees) in the initial trial block. This effect was much shallower in later blocks. He proposed that the initial effect may indicate that mental rotation is the default process for recognising rotated objects, and that the decrease in this effect, seen with practice, may reflect the increased use of learned orientation-invariant features. Initially, we were interested in whether object-colour associations that may be learned during the initial block, could account for the reduced latency to name rotated objects, seen in later blocks. In experiment 1 we used full-cue colour images of objects that depicted colour and other surface cues. Surprisingly, given that Jolicoeur's findings were replicated several times with line drawings, we found that even the initial linear trend in naming latency was shallow. We replicated this result in follow-up experiments. In contrast, when we used less-realistic depictions of the same objects that had fewer visual cues (ie line drawings, coloured drawings, greyscale images), the results were comparable to those of Jolicoeur. Also, the initial linear trends were steeper for these depictions than for full-cue colour images. The results suggest that, when multiple surface cues are available in the image, mental rotation may not be the default recognition process.  相似文献   
267.
This study examines the effects of a verbal collateral (counting) on a series of three differential reinforcement of low rates of response (DRL) tasks with progressively longer interresponse time requirements (DRL-2, DRL-6 and DRL-12). Forty-eight 10-year-old children, divided according to sex and conceptual tempo (fast-inaccurate vs. slow-accurate) participated in DRL training, half of them being instructed to count aloud between responses. Counting was found to be related to faster learning of all the tasks and to greater efficiency in obtaining reinforcement in most cases. It also eliminated differences due to sex and conceptual tempo except in DRL-12. However, it had the disadvantage of causing inexact temporal discrimination. The results were replicated in a group of 8-year-old subjects performing the same tasks.  相似文献   
268.
269.
270.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号