全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1514篇 |
免费 | 46篇 |
专业分类
1560篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 6篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 24篇 |
2021年 | 17篇 |
2020年 | 30篇 |
2019年 | 44篇 |
2018年 | 60篇 |
2017年 | 79篇 |
2016年 | 47篇 |
2015年 | 35篇 |
2014年 | 51篇 |
2013年 | 172篇 |
2012年 | 77篇 |
2011年 | 87篇 |
2010年 | 39篇 |
2009年 | 64篇 |
2008年 | 65篇 |
2007年 | 58篇 |
2006年 | 55篇 |
2005年 | 71篇 |
2004年 | 50篇 |
2003年 | 49篇 |
2002年 | 38篇 |
2001年 | 17篇 |
2000年 | 20篇 |
1999年 | 26篇 |
1998年 | 18篇 |
1997年 | 12篇 |
1996年 | 13篇 |
1995年 | 13篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 11篇 |
1992年 | 13篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 12篇 |
1984年 | 10篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
1972年 | 6篇 |
1970年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有1560条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
851.
Nicholas Griffin 《Synthese》2018,195(12):5521-5547
Brandom’s inferentialism offers, in many ways, a radically new approach to old issues in semantics and the theory of intentionality. But, in one respect at least, it clings tenaciously to the mainstream philosophical tradition of the middle years of the twentieth century. Against the theory’s natural tendencies, Brandom aligns it with the ’linguistic turn’ that philosophy took in the middle of the last century by insisting, in the face of considerable opposing evidence, that intentionality is the preserve of those who can offer and ask for reasons and thus of language users alone. In this paper, I argue that there is no good reason for giving inferentialism a linguistic twist, and that, in doing so, Brandom is forced to make claims which are implausible in themselves and lead him, in the attempt to mitigate them, to a number of doubtful expedients. 相似文献
852.
853.
854.
855.
Allison Levy Iser G. DeLeon Catherine K. Martinez Nathalie Fernandez Nicholas A. Gage Sigurdur Óli Sigurdsson Michelle A. Frank‐Crawford 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2017,50(2):206-221
The overjustification hypothesis suggests that extrinsic rewards undermine intrinsic motivation. Extrinsic rewards are common in strengthening behavior in persons with intellectual and developmental disabilities; we examined overjustification effects in this context. A literature search yielded 65 data sets permitting comparison of responding during an initial no‐reinforcement phase to a subsequent no‐reinforcement phase, separated by a reinforcement phase. We used effect sizes to compare response levels in these two no‐reinforcement phases. Overall, the mean effect size did not differ from zero; levels in the second no‐reinforcement phase were equally likely to be higher or lower than in the first. However, in contrast to the overjustification hypothesis, levels were higher in the second no‐reinforcement phase when comparing the single no‐reinforcement sessions immediately before and after reinforcement. Outcomes consistent with the overjustification hypothesis were somewhat more likely when the target behavior occurred at relatively higher levels prior to reinforcement. 相似文献
856.
Recent scholarship has discovered significant racial/ethnic group variation in response to political threats such as immigration and terrorism. Surprisingly, minority groups often simultaneously perceive themselves to be at greater risk from such threats and yet still prefer more open immigration policies and civil liberties protections. We suggest a group‐level empathy process may explain this puzzle: Due to their higher levels of empathy for other disadvantaged groups, many minority group members support protections for others even when their own interests are threatened. Little is known, however, about the unique properties of group empathy or its role in policy opinion formation. In this study, we examine the reliability and validity of our new measure of group empathy, the Group Empathy Index (GEI), demonstrating that it is distinct from other social and political predispositions such as ethnocentrism, social dominance orientation, authoritarianism, ideology, and partisanship. We then propose a theory about the development of group empathy in reaction to life experiences based on one's race/ethnicity, gender, age, and education. Finally, we examine the power of group empathy to predict policy attitudes and political behavior. 相似文献
857.
858.
Phia S. Salter Nicholas J. Kelley Ludwin E. Molina Luyen T. Thai 《Memory (Hove, England)》2017,25(8):1139-1147
Photographs provide critical retrieval cues for personal remembering, but few studies have considered this phenomenon at the collective level. In this research, we examined the psychological consequences of visual attention to the presence (or absence) of racially charged retrieval cues within American racial segregation photographs. We hypothesised that attention to racial retrieval cues embedded in historical photographs would increase social justice concept accessibility. In Study 1, we recorded gaze patterns with an eye-tracker among participants viewing images that contained racial retrieval cues or were digitally manipulated to remove them. In Study 2, we manipulated participants’ gaze behaviour by either directing visual attention toward racial retrieval cues, away from racial retrieval cues, or directing attention within photographs where racial retrieval cues were missing. Across Studies 1 and 2, visual attention to racial retrieval cues in photographs documenting historical segregation predicted social justice concept accessibility. 相似文献
859.
Nicholas Stanger Maria Kavussanu Christopher Ring 《Journal of Applied Sport Psychology》2017,29(1):44-58
This research investigated whether gender moderates, and anger mediates, the relationship between empathy (i.e., perspective taking and empathic concern) and aggressiveness in sport. In Study 1, perspective taking and empathic concern were negatively associated with aggressiveness, and this effect was stronger in women compared to men. In Study 2, perspective taking was a negative predictor of aggressiveness and antisocial behavior in sport, and anger mediated these relationships in women but not in men. Our findings suggest that empathy and emotion-based strategies targeted at reducing aggressiveness in sport need to be tailored for male and female athletes. 相似文献
860.
Brittany G. Travers Erin D. Bigler Tyler C. Duffield Molly D.B. Prigge Alyson L. Froehlich Nicholas Lange Andrew L. Alexander Janet E. Lainhart 《Developmental science》2017,20(4)
Many individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) exhibit motor difficulties, but it is unknown whether manual motor skills improve, plateau, or decline in ASD in the transition from childhood into adulthood. Atypical development of manual motor skills could impact the ability to learn and perform daily activities across the life span. This study examined longitudinal grip strength and finger tapping development in individuals with ASD (n = 90) compared to individuals with typical development (n = 56), ages 5 to 40 years old. We further examined manual motor performance as a possible correlate of current and future daily living skills. The group with ASD demonstrated atypical motor development, characterized by similar performance during childhood but increasingly poorer performance from adolescence into adulthood. Grip strength was correlated with current adaptive daily living skills, and Time 1 grip strength predicted daily living skills eight years into the future. These results suggest that individuals with ASD may experience increasingly more pronounced motor difficulties from adolescence into adulthood and that manual motor performance in ASD is related to adaptive daily living skills. 相似文献