全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1461篇 |
免费 | 45篇 |
专业分类
1506篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 6篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 23篇 |
2021年 | 17篇 |
2020年 | 30篇 |
2019年 | 43篇 |
2018年 | 59篇 |
2017年 | 78篇 |
2016年 | 48篇 |
2015年 | 34篇 |
2014年 | 50篇 |
2013年 | 166篇 |
2012年 | 76篇 |
2011年 | 83篇 |
2010年 | 38篇 |
2009年 | 58篇 |
2008年 | 64篇 |
2007年 | 57篇 |
2006年 | 53篇 |
2005年 | 72篇 |
2004年 | 48篇 |
2003年 | 46篇 |
2002年 | 37篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 16篇 |
1999年 | 23篇 |
1998年 | 18篇 |
1997年 | 12篇 |
1996年 | 12篇 |
1995年 | 12篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 11篇 |
1992年 | 12篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 11篇 |
1984年 | 10篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
1972年 | 6篇 |
1970年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有1506条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
261.
262.
Nicholas P. Sarantakis 《Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy》2017,47(2):135-140
Challenges and opportunities for psychologists and psychotherapists in respect to explicit and implicit discrimination issues in therapy are explored, both from the side of the therapist and the client. Furthermore, personal reflections on such issues are discussed drawing on examples of indirect discrimination on the basis of race and sexual orientation. It is suggested that a combination of professional anti-discriminatory guidelines, a willingness to understand deeply the client’s frame of reference and self-reflection can guard against such phenomena that can harm ethical and constructive psychotherapy. 相似文献
263.
Logically, responding aggressively to rejection is maladaptive because one is unlikely to seek a relationship with an aggressor. We predict that when concealed, the illogical aggressive response to rejection is more likely, whereas when the rejected individuals’ aggressive responses are perceived as public, the aggressive acts may be reduced. Participants were rejected by others (Experiment 1) or were either accepted or rejected during an online ball-tossing game (Experiment 2) and were then given an opportunity to aggress publicly or privately. Across experiments, when the opportunity to aggress was made public, rejected participants exhibited less aggressive behavior. When concerned about the perception of their public aggressive responses by others, rejected individuals’ aggressive responses diminished compared with those whose actions were private. Crucially, this extended to aggression visible only to neutral others, suggesting that effects cannot solely be due to fear of retribution. 相似文献
264.
Nicholas J. Eastman Morgan Anderson Deron Boyles 《Studies in Philosophy and Education》2017,36(1):61-81
Simply put, charter schools have not lived up to their advocates’ promise of equity. Using examples of tangible civil rights gains of the twentieth century (e.g. Brown v. Board, Lau v. Nichols) and extending feminist theories of invisible labor to include the labor of democracy, the authors argue that the charter movement renders invisible the labor that secured civil protections for historically marginalized groups. The charter movement hangs a quality public education—previously recognized as a universal guarantee—on the education consumer’s ability to navigate a marketplace. The authors conclude that the neoliberal agenda of positioning choice as the best mechanism for securing an education rolls back the rights that were already secured through the labor of democracy. 相似文献
265.
Jeffrey M. Cucina Nicholas L. Vasilopoulos Kashi Sehgal 《Journal of business and psychology》2005,20(2):275-290
This study tested the hypothesis that personality-based job analysis (PBJA) ratings are correlated with subject matter expert
(SME) personality scale scores (through a self-serving bias). Some support was found for the hypothesis. However, PBJA ratings
were somewhat predictive of the criterion-related validity of scale scores for the big five personality dimensions. 相似文献
266.
We explored the influence of negation on cognitive dynamics, measured using mouse-movement trajectories, to test the classic notion that negation acts as an operator on linguistic processing. In three experiments, participants verified the truth or falsity of simple statements, and we tracked the computer-mouse trajectories of their responses. Sentences expressing these facts sometimes contained a negation. Such negated statements could be true (e.g., "elephants are not small") or false (e.g., "elephants are not large"). In the first experiment, as predicted by the classic notion of negation, we found that negation caused more discreteness in the mouse trajectory of a response. The second experiment induced a simple context for these statements, yet negation still increased discreteness in trajectories. A third experiment enhanced the pragmatic context of sentences, and the discreteness was substantially diminished, with one primary measure no longer significantly showing increased discreteness at all. Traditional linguistic theories predict rapid shifts in cognitive dynamics occur due to the nature of negation: It is an operator that reverses the truth or falsity of an interpretation. We argue that these results support both propositional and contextual accounts of negation present in the literature, suggesting that contextual factors are crucial for determining the kind of cognitive dynamics displayed. We conclude by drawing broader lessons about theories of cognition from the case of negation. 相似文献
267.
Melikşah Demir Ayça Özen Aysun Doğan Nicholas A. Bilyk Fanita A. Tyrell 《Journal of Happiness Studies》2011,12(6):983-1005
Decades of empirical research have shown that friendship experiences are an essential predictor of happiness. However, what might account for the relationship between friendship and happiness? Two studies investigated perceived mattering (Marshall, J Adolesc 24:473–490, 2001) as a mediator of the association between friendship quality and happiness. Study 1 showed that perceived mattering to one’s best friend mediated the relationship between friendship and happiness. Study 2 replicated the findings of the first study and showed that mattering in friendships accounts for the role of friendship quality in happiness across the three closest friendships of the individual. The results are discussed in terms of the theoretical importance of understanding how friendship is related to happiness. 相似文献
268.
Despite its theoretical importance, personal goal motivation has rarely been examined in clinical depression. Here we investigate whether clinically depressed persons (n = 23) differ from never-depressed persons (n = 26) on number of freely generated approach and avoidance goals, appraisals of these goals, and reasons why these goals would and would not be achieved. Participants listed approach and avoidance goals separately and generated explanations for why they would (pro) and would not (con) achieve their most important approach and avoidance goals, before rating the importance, likelihood, and perceived control of goal outcomes. Counter to hypothesis, depressed persons did not differ from never-depressed controls on number of approach or avoidance goals, or on the perceived importance of these goals. However, compared to never-depressed controls, depressed individuals gave lower likelihood judgments for desirable approach goal outcomes, tended to give higher likelihood judgments for undesirable to-be-avoided goal outcomes, and gave lower ratings of their control over goal outcomes. Furthermore, although controls generated significantly more pro than con reasons for goal achievement, depressed participants did not. These results suggest that depressed persons do not lack valued goals but are more pessimistic about their likelihood, controllability, and reasons for successful goal attainment. 相似文献
269.
Compulsive hoarding is a chronic and debilitating condition that has proven difficult to treat. The cognitive behavioral approaches that have otherwise been successful for people suffering from obsessive-compulsive disorder do not work very well for hoarding (Abramowitz, Franklin, Schwartz, & Furr, 2003). Many have suggested that this is due to the ego-syntonic nature of the symptoms and the accompanying lack of insight. This paper describes several challenges related to insight that limit the effectiveness of traditional cognitive behavioral approaches for compulsive hoarding. Problems with anosognosia, overvalued ideation, and defensiveness that characterize hoarding are described and suggestions offered for dealing with them. 相似文献
270.
Nicholas G. Evans 《Nanoethics》2010,4(3):215-220
In this paper, I will discuss the responsibilities that scientists have for ensuring their work is interpreted correctly.
I will argue that there are three good reasons for scientists to work to ensure the appropriate communication of their findings.
First, I will argue that scientists have a general obligation to ensure scientific research is communicated properly based
on the vulnerability of others to the misrepresentation of their work. Second, I will argue that scientists have a special
obligation to do so because of the power we as a society invest in them as specialists and professionals. Finally, I will
argue that scientists ought to ensure their work is interpreted correctly based on prudential, self-interested considerations.
I will conclude by offering suggestions regarding policy considerations. 相似文献