首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   196篇
  免费   9篇
  2020年   2篇
  2019年   5篇
  2018年   3篇
  2017年   8篇
  2015年   3篇
  2014年   4篇
  2013年   20篇
  2012年   5篇
  2011年   4篇
  2010年   6篇
  2008年   3篇
  2007年   8篇
  2006年   4篇
  2004年   3篇
  2003年   5篇
  2002年   5篇
  2001年   3篇
  1999年   1篇
  1998年   3篇
  1997年   5篇
  1995年   4篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   5篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   6篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   1篇
  1988年   5篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   4篇
  1985年   1篇
  1984年   5篇
  1983年   4篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   2篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   1篇
  1978年   1篇
  1977年   5篇
  1976年   4篇
  1975年   2篇
  1973年   2篇
  1972年   2篇
  1971年   6篇
  1970年   5篇
  1969年   3篇
  1968年   1篇
  1967年   4篇
  1966年   4篇
  1954年   1篇
排序方式: 共有205条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
141.
142.
143.
144.
We have shown that prominent changes in heart rate (HR) and blood pressure (BP) occur in dogs during classical defensive conditioning. These changes are usually analyzed as average HR and BP during control periods versus averages during conditional stimulus (CS) and unconditional stimulus (US) periods. A new method has been developed for evaluation of these functions, viz., analysis across many trials of successive beats beginning at onset of CS and working backward (control period) and forward (CS period). Nine awake dogs were continuously studied: six with HR and intra-arterial BP measurements and three with HR measurements only. A drop in HR occurs 1 to 2 beats after CS onset (often noted as a “dropped” beat on individual trials). In some dogs, on the third to fifth beat HR reaches its maximum during the CS period, thereafter falling slightly, but still above baseline; other dogs show progressively increasing HR throughout the whole CS period. At US onset (foreleg shock) HR usually rises rapidly to a peak greater than that at CS maximum. BP follows HR changes fairly closely, diastolic better than systolic. These results indicate that I) this method can reveal transients previously unsuspected, 2) a prominent initial sudden bradycardia followed by tachycardia commonly occurs in conditioning in dogs, 3) the latent period of prominent HR increase is usually quite short (1.3 to 1.6 secs.), and 4) BP follows HR directly during the early part of the CS.  相似文献   
145.
Eight inbred beagle dogs, litter mates of two generations, exhibited psychogenic vagotonia (long sinus pauses and transient atrioventricular block of the heart) during orienting to tones. This was intensified during tones following “one-trial conditioning” to a single painful leg-shock. It was potentiated during conditional stimuli (CS+) regularly reinforced by leg-shock; whereas unreinforced tones (CS?) elicited less vagotonia. Additional evidence of differentiation was observed in one dog when the differential magnitudes of the responses were reversed following reversal of the significance of the conditional stimuli. The possible mechanisms of these changes and correlations with clinical material are discussed. Results of this study illustrate the interplay between constitutional predisposition and psychological factors in prominent cardiovascular changes.  相似文献   
146.
147.
The mathematical concept of pragmatic truth, first introduced in Mikenberg, da Costa and Chuaqui (1986), has received in the last few years several applications in logic and the philosophy of science. In this paper, we study the logic of pragmatic truth, and show that there are important connections between this logic, modal logic and, in particular, Jaskowski's discussive logic. In order to do so, two systems are put forward so that the notions of pragmatic validity and pragmatic truth can be accommodated. One of the main results of this paper is that the logic of pragmatic truth is paraconsistent. The philosophical import of this result, which justifies the application of pragmatic truth to inconsistent settings, is also discussed.  相似文献   
148.
Studies of behavioral traits of the “Arkansas Line of nervous pointer dogs” are well documented (Murphreeet al., 1969). Demonstrations of drug effects on these traits, as measured on the standardized behavior test and expressed as “Morbidity Score,” have been disappointing, despite other evidence of effectiveness of drug treatments such as modification of operant performance (Murphreeet al. 1974c). A simple reproducible test with high interrater reliability is presented here. Subjects were nine nervous dogs that received placebo, pimozide, and chlordiazepoxide in a double-blind crossover design. Both drugs significantly attenuated “human avoidance,” a nervous trait. Placebo had no effect. The chlordiazepoxide effect was maximal on the first day of administration, returning to baseline three to four days after the last dose. The pimozide maximal effect was on day 4, with return to baseline six to nine days after the last dose. In addition to the aberrant behavior of the nervous pointer, hyperresponsiveness to dopaminergic stimulation and increased levels of L-DOPA in the cisternal spinal fluid were observed.  相似文献   
149.
150.
Everyday environments, even small regions within reach, vary dramatically in terms of material composition. Adapting one's manual behavior to such transitions can be considered to be an important element of skilled action. To investigate the origins of this ability, we presented 8-month-olds (n=24) and 10-month-olds (n=24) hard or soft objects on a composite tabletop substrate that was half rigid and half flexible. Results indicated infants explored the objects selectively and geared their manual behaviors, with or without an object in hand, to the particular substrate they contacted. More broadly, the study suggests that infant manual exploration is flexible even in the face of abrupt transitions in material structure. Such flexibility may support early attempts at problem solving and tool use.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号