首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   33773篇
  免费   1272篇
  国内免费   19篇
  35064篇
  2020年   326篇
  2019年   450篇
  2018年   610篇
  2017年   672篇
  2016年   655篇
  2015年   466篇
  2014年   542篇
  2013年   2649篇
  2012年   936篇
  2011年   1069篇
  2010年   697篇
  2009年   693篇
  2008年   982篇
  2007年   919篇
  2006年   868篇
  2005年   732篇
  2004年   674篇
  2003年   726篇
  2002年   674篇
  2001年   1233篇
  2000年   1201篇
  1999年   864篇
  1998年   397篇
  1997年   327篇
  1996年   326篇
  1995年   320篇
  1994年   318篇
  1992年   738篇
  1991年   712篇
  1990年   669篇
  1989年   630篇
  1988年   605篇
  1987年   591篇
  1986年   557篇
  1985年   628篇
  1984年   499篇
  1983年   377篇
  1979年   478篇
  1978年   354篇
  1977年   339篇
  1976年   326篇
  1975年   420篇
  1974年   430篇
  1973年   488篇
  1972年   386篇
  1971年   323篇
  1969年   328篇
  1968年   380篇
  1967年   334篇
  1966年   385篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
102.
The startle probe response: a new measure of emotion?   总被引:17,自引:0,他引:17  
  相似文献   
103.
104.
On second-order family therapy   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
S Golann 《Family process》1988,27(1):51-71
If family therapists remain aware of the indivisible and recursive nature of their interactions with families, and if they use this awareness to form a collaborative rather than a hierarchical therapeutic system, and at the same time minimize their attempts to change persons or family structures in strategic or predetermined ways, then they may be said to be practicing a "second-order" family therapy. This article analyzes the development of the second-order position on therapist power and influence, concluding that it is inconsistent and possibly disingenuous.  相似文献   
105.
106.
"Without a theory of mind one cannot participate in a conversation"   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
S Baron-Cohen 《Cognition》1988,29(1):83-84
  相似文献   
107.
A longitudinal design was employed to test the main and stress-moderating effects of young adolescents' perceived family environment (Family Environment Scales; FES; Moos & Moos, 1981) on their depression, anxiety, and self-esteem. This study was part of a larger longitudinal project (L. Cohen, Burt, & Bjorck, 1987) that demonstrated the significant cross-sectional effects of the young adolescents' controllable and uncontrollable negative events, and the significant longitudinal effects of the former. The present cross-sectional analyses demonstrated the hypothesized main effects of the FES scores; families perceived as cohesive, organized, and expressive were related to positive psychological functioning, whereas families perceived as conflict-ridden and controlling were related to negative functioning. However, in general these effects were nonsignificant in the longitudinal analyses. Although there were a number of significant Negative Events x FES interactions, in no instance did the pattern support the hypothesized stress-buffering role of positive family climate.  相似文献   
108.
A frequently advocated strategy for increasing the efficiency of child abuse prevention programs is to deliver prevention services to "high-risk" populations. This article critically reviews procedures for the reliable and valid assessment of child abuse potential within an ecological perspective. Factors that limit the usefulness of child abuse risk assessment are discussed. These factors include the uncertain criteria of child abuse and neglect, the low base rate of the phenomenon, and the financial and social costs of such procedures. Finally, the prevention implications of the current and future state of the art in child abuse risk assessment are considered and preventive interventions that do not depend on individual case risk screening are advocated.  相似文献   
109.
An experiment that examined the way in which young readers deployed eye movements while reading sentences and while answering questions containing either a pronominal or noun anaphor is reported. To evaluate the possible causal role played by differences in inspection strategies between readers of above- and below-average reading skill, a third“age control” group of younger children was also tested. This group was matched on absolute reading ability with the less skilled group of older children, and on relative reading ability (i.e. reading quotient) with the more skilled group. Differences in inspection strategy were apparent between the groups of good and poor readers. Good readers launched more selective reinspections, whereas the poorer readers were more inclined to engage in“backtracking” and appeared to make less use of the displayed text. In every case there was a marked similarity in the behaviour of the good readers and the“age controls”. These results suggest that the ability to code the spatial location of words in a sentence, and, where necessary, to use this information to launch accurately targetted selective reinspections of previously read text, plays a crucial role in the development of skilled reading performance.  相似文献   
110.
The distorted room illusion (DRI) and the attendant argument for perceptual ambiguity is critically analyzed from a Gibsonian/ecological point of view. The notions of multiple specification, conflicting information, and perceptual skill are invoked in showing how the ecological approach can accommodate illusion effects that may remain under mobile binocular viewing conditions. Static optic arrays are shown not to be ambiguous. So-called equivalent configurations are found to be analytic artifacts, appearing when the problem of information is treated in geometrical terms without regard for constraints due to physical and ecological regularities. The relative importance of motion-based and motion-independent information is discussed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号