全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1131篇 |
免费 | 17篇 |
专业分类
1148篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 11篇 |
2020年 | 12篇 |
2019年 | 25篇 |
2018年 | 29篇 |
2017年 | 24篇 |
2016年 | 36篇 |
2015年 | 28篇 |
2014年 | 18篇 |
2013年 | 93篇 |
2012年 | 39篇 |
2011年 | 47篇 |
2010年 | 20篇 |
2009年 | 20篇 |
2008年 | 28篇 |
2007年 | 36篇 |
2006年 | 28篇 |
2005年 | 35篇 |
2004年 | 30篇 |
2003年 | 40篇 |
2002年 | 26篇 |
2001年 | 17篇 |
2000年 | 33篇 |
1999年 | 26篇 |
1998年 | 12篇 |
1997年 | 17篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1995年 | 21篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 19篇 |
1990年 | 18篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 15篇 |
1987年 | 22篇 |
1986年 | 13篇 |
1985年 | 13篇 |
1984年 | 16篇 |
1983年 | 8篇 |
1982年 | 8篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 15篇 |
1978年 | 21篇 |
1977年 | 15篇 |
1976年 | 12篇 |
1975年 | 20篇 |
1974年 | 17篇 |
1973年 | 14篇 |
1972年 | 9篇 |
1971年 | 10篇 |
1966年 | 13篇 |
排序方式: 共有1148条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
231.
Four experiments were conducted to study the bias of perceived length for Müller-Lyer configurations that contained a single set of fins (i.e., two segments that join to form a vertex). The experiments manipulated several factors that have been shown to be critical to the effect: (1) version (which way the apex pointed), (2) length of the stimulus span, (3) presence or absence of a line segment in the span being judged, (4) fin length, (5) fin angle, and (6) the zone in which the response was rendered. Using percent error as the index of perceptual distortion, the major finding was that the two versions show an opposite slope for strength of effect as a function of span. When stimulus spans were plotted against response means (not converting to percent error), an almost perfect linear relation was found. These results indicate that the perceptual effects can be modeled as a linear system having two parameters through which the treatments exert their influence. The results are discussed in relation to major theories of mechanism for the Müller-Lyer illusion. 相似文献
232.
Jams S. Nelson 《Zygon》1995,30(2):267-280
Abstract. The concept of God's acting in the world has been seen to be problematic in light of the claims of scientific knowledge that the regularity of a law like universe rules out divine action. There are resources in both scientific knowledge and religion that can render meaningful and credible divine action. The new physics, chaos theory, cognitive psychology, and the concept of top-down causation are used to understand how God acts in the world. God's action is not an intervention, but is understood on the model of how the mind influences the brain in a downward causative manner. Suggestions for imagining God's actions are discussed. 相似文献
233.
This study addressed the question: Do special education labels bias teachers' evaluations of children? Thirty primary level teachers were given different labels for the same child: learning disabled, educable mentally retarded, emotionally disturbed, normal, or no label. Teachers made behavioral observations from the same video-tape, completed a checklist, and graded the same academic work purportedly completed by the target child. Teachers' behavioral observations and grading of the academic work were not influenced by the special education labels, but the checklist scores were. Implications for the philosophical debate about the effects of labeling and for methods of classroom data collection by teachers are discussed. 相似文献
234.
235.
Interference generated among the sensory features of PA stimulus words is not eliminated by processing at the imaginal level. This experiment was planned to test the reverse possibility. Will interference generated by similar imaginal referents be eliminated by sensory overlap between the stimuli and their responses? Thus, stimulus words did or did not evoke similar images, pairs did or did not share initial letters, and lists were acquired either under an instructional set t o form interacting images or to repeat the items aloud. The results indicated that the imagery strategy was superior to the repetition strategy in all conditions, and that stimulus interference was statistically eliminated when pairs shared the same initial letters. The findings were interpreted as consistent with a levels of processing conceptualization. 相似文献
236.
237.
Previous findings indicate that natural category size affects cued recall but not recognition performance. Words that define or belong to larger categories are not as likely to be recalled in the presence of an extralist cue. However, category size has no effect on recognition in the presence of the target as the cue. Theoretically, this difference could be due to inherent differences between these tasks, to the use of different types of test cues, or to differences in the nature of the required responses (naming compared with “yes/no”decisions). Three experiments indicated that none of these factors is a sole determinant. Natural category size effects were found in cued-recall and recognition tasks, with extralist and target cues and regardless of the required response. The critical factor is whether the testing conditions require or encourage subjects to search the category defined by the cue. With the initiation of such a search, information represented in semantic memory is likely to influence memory for episodic information. 相似文献
238.
Douglas L. Nelson Cathy L. McEvoy John R. Walling Joseph W. Wheeler 《Behavior research methods》1980,12(1):16-37
Norms were collected to determine the relative dominance of different meanings of homo-graphic words. Forty-six subjects wrote down the first word that came to mind for each of 320 homographs. Each homograph, the number of times each meaning was given, and the specific associates are made available. In addition, correlations with other norms are presented. 相似文献
239.
240.