全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1067篇 |
免费 | 16篇 |
专业分类
1083篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 23篇 |
2019年 | 21篇 |
2018年 | 23篇 |
2017年 | 23篇 |
2016年 | 40篇 |
2015年 | 27篇 |
2014年 | 32篇 |
2013年 | 102篇 |
2012年 | 46篇 |
2011年 | 46篇 |
2010年 | 30篇 |
2009年 | 42篇 |
2008年 | 51篇 |
2007年 | 62篇 |
2006年 | 50篇 |
2005年 | 40篇 |
2004年 | 37篇 |
2003年 | 47篇 |
2002年 | 46篇 |
2001年 | 14篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 19篇 |
1998年 | 15篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 15篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 11篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 9篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 11篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 10篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有1083条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
881.
Anthony J. Ryals Jonathan T. O’Neil M.-Marsel Mesulam Sandra Weintraub Joel L. Voss 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2019,26(4):577-598
Successful memory is normally accompanied by explicit awareness of retrieval and confidence in the accuracy of the retrieval product. Prior findings suggest that these features of metamemory can be dissociated from retrieval accuracy in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment (aMCI). However, the literature on this question contains variable and conflicting results, likely because of differences in experimental conditions. We sought to systematically evaluate memory awareness disruptions in aMCI using multiple measures and stimulus formats within the same individuals. Memory awareness was tested with global predictions and postdictions, judgments of learning, confidence level ratings, and modified feeling-of-knowing ratings in tasks of visuospatial and verbal memory. These tests were administered to 14 individuals with aMCI and 15 healthy, age-matched controls. Memory awareness accuracy was calculated as the correspondence between subjective judgments and memory performance.Individuals with aMCI demonstrated impaired global and trial-level retrospective task awareness for visuospatial and verbal stimuli. Additionally, modified feeling-of-knowing awareness was impaired selectively for verbal stimuli. Statistical effect sizes for global awareness impairments were comparable to impairments in several objective neuropsychological memory assessments.Memory awareness (metamemory) disruptions in aMCI were most evident for a subset of subjective judgment types and task input modalities. These findings advance understanding of the nature of memory impairments in aMCI and support the utility of incorporating memory awareness testing to better characterize memory integrity in older adults. 相似文献
882.
883.
884.
Neil McMillan Allison H. Hahn Jenna V. Congdon Kimberley A. Campbell John Hoang Erin N. Scully Marcia L. Spetch Christopher B. Sturdy 《Animal cognition》2017,20(4):655-663
Chickadees are high-metabolism, non-migratory birds, and thus an especially interesting model for studying how animals follow patterns of food availability over time. Here, we studied whether black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) could learn to reverse their behavior and/or to anticipate changes in reinforcement when the reinforcer contingencies for each stimulus were not stably fixed in time. In Experiment 1, we examined the responses of chickadees on an auditory go/no-go task, with constant reversals in reinforcement contingencies every 120 trials across daily testing intervals. Chickadees did not produce above-chance discrimination; however, when trained with a procedure that only reversed after successful discrimination, chickadees were able to discriminate and reverse their behavior successfully. In Experiment 2, we examined the responses of chickadees when reversals were structured to occur at the same time once per day, and chickadees were again able to discriminate and reverse their behavior over time, though they showed no reliable evidence of reversal anticipation. The frequency of reversals throughout the day thus appears to be an important determinant for these animals’ performance in reversal procedures. 相似文献
885.
Neil Bolton 《Psychologie appliquee》1993,42(3):229-231
886.
887.
Lisa M. Toole Lynn G. Bowman Jessica L. Thomason Louis P. Hagopian Karena S. Rush 《Behavioral Interventions》2003,18(1):35-42
A 15‐year‐old female diagnosed with mental retardation was admitted to an inpatient facility for the assessment and treatment of a severe behavior disorder. A behavioral treatment to reduce destructive behavior consisting of enriched environment, differential reinforcement, and time‐out was evaluated in a reversal design. Data on affect were collected during baseline and treatment phases. In addition to achieving clinically significant reductions in problem behavior, collateral improvements in positive affect were observed during treatment phases. Copyright © 2003 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
888.
Bruce M. Hood C. Neil Macrae Victoria Cole‐Davies Melanie Dias 《Developmental science》2003,6(1):67-71
Children and adults were tested on a forced‐choice face recognition task in which the direction of eye gaze was manipulated over the course of the initial presentation and subsequent test phase of the experiment. To establish the effects of gaze direction on the encoding process, participants were presented with to‐be‐studied faces displaying either direct or deviated gaze (i.e. encoding manipulation). At test, all the faces depicted persons with their eyes closed. To investigate the effects of gaze direction on the efficiency of the retrieval process, a second condition (i.e. retrieval manipulation) was run in which target faces were presented initially with eyes closed and tested with either direct or deviated gaze. The results revealed the encoding advantages enjoyed by faces with direct gaze was present for both children and adults. Faces with direct gaze were also recognized better than faces with deviated gaze at retrieval, although this effect was most pronounced for adults. Finally, the advantage for direct gaze over deviated gaze at encoding was greater than the advantage for direct gaze over deviated gaze at retrieval. We consider the theoretical implications of these findings. 相似文献
889.
Oliver C. Mudford Neil T. Martin Jasmine K. Y. Hui Sarah Ann Taylor 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2009,42(3):527-539
The three algorithms most frequently selected by behavior‐analytic researchers to compute interobserver agreement with continuous recording were used to assess the accuracy of data recorded from video samples on handheld computers by 12 observers. Rate and duration of responding were recorded for three samples each. Data files were compared with criterion records to determine observer accuracy. Block‐by‐block and exact agreement algorithms were susceptible to inflated agreement and accuracy estimates at lower rates and durations. The exact agreement method appeared to be overly stringent for recording responding at higher rates (23.5 responses per minute) and for higher relative duration (72% of session). Time‐window analysis appeared to inflate accuracy assessment at relatively high but not at low response rate and duration (4.8 responses per minute and 8% of session, respectively). 相似文献
890.
Neil Levy 《Theoretical medicine and bioethics》2018,39(2):123-141
Proposals for regulating or nudging healthy choices are controversial. Opponents often argue that individuals should take responsibility for their own health, rather than be paternalistically manipulated for their own good. In this paper, I argue that people can take responsibility for their own health only if they satisfy certain epistemic conditions, but we live in an epistemic environment in which these conditions are not satisfied. Satisfying the epistemic conditions for taking responsibility, I argue, requires regulation of this environment. I describe some proposals for such regulation and show that we cannot reject all regulation in the name of individual responsibility. We must either regulate individuals’ healthy choices or regulate the epistemic environment. 相似文献