全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1044篇 |
免费 | 15篇 |
专业分类
1059篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 22篇 |
2019年 | 20篇 |
2018年 | 22篇 |
2017年 | 23篇 |
2016年 | 40篇 |
2015年 | 27篇 |
2014年 | 32篇 |
2013年 | 107篇 |
2012年 | 47篇 |
2011年 | 44篇 |
2010年 | 29篇 |
2009年 | 43篇 |
2008年 | 51篇 |
2007年 | 61篇 |
2006年 | 49篇 |
2005年 | 39篇 |
2004年 | 37篇 |
2003年 | 47篇 |
2002年 | 47篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 9篇 |
1999年 | 19篇 |
1998年 | 15篇 |
1997年 | 9篇 |
1996年 | 15篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 7篇 |
1992年 | 11篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 7篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 12篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 10篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有1059条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
The authors examined participants' estimates of own and parental psychometric intelligence (IQ) and emotional intelligence (EI). The authors asked 224 participants (82 men, 138 women, 4 people who did not report their gender) to estimate their own and their parents' IQ and EI scores on a normal distribution ranging from 55 to 145 points. The authors hypothesized that men would give higher IQ but lower EI self-estimates than women and that participants, regardless of gender, would rate their fathers as higher on IQ but lower on EI than their mothers. The results confirmed the hypotheses, supporting the view that people perceive psychometric intelligence as a primarily masculine attribute in contrast with emotional intelligence, which they perceive as a primarily feminine attribute. The results also showed that the intensity of the stereotypical perception of EI as a feminine attribute diminished when the authors asked participants to estimate their scores on a range of specific EI facets instead of providing a direct overall self-estimate. 相似文献
52.
Guided by a heuristic account of social-cognitive functioning, researchers have attempted to identify the cognitive benefits that derive from a categorical approach to person construal. While revealing, this work has overlooked the fact that, prior to the application of categorical thinking as an economizing mental tool, perceivers must first extract category-triggering information from available stimulus cues. It is possible, therefore, that basic perceptual processes may also contribute to people's propensity to view others in a category-based manner. This possibility was explored in 3 experiments in which the authors investigated the ease with which perceivers can extract categorical and identity-based knowledge from faces under both optimal and suboptimal (i.e., inverted faces, blurred faces, rapidly presented faces) processing conditions. The results confirmed that categorical knowledge is extracted from faces more efficiently than identity-related knowledge, a finding that underscores the importance of perceptual operations in the generation of categorical thinking. 相似文献
53.
Gaze direction is a vital communicative channel through which people transmit information to each other. By signaling the locus of social attention, gaze cues convey information about the relative importance of objects, including other people, in the environment. For the most part, this information is communicated via patterns of gaze direction, with gaze shifts signaling changes in the objects of attention. Noting the relevance of gaze cues in social cognition, we speculated that gaze shifts may modulate people's evaluations of others. We investigated this possibility by asking participants to judge the likability (Experiment 1) and physical attractiveness (Experiment 2) of targets displaying gaze shifts indicative of attentional engagement or disengagement with the participants. As expected, person evaluation was moderated by the direction of gaze shifts, but only when the judgment under consideration was relevant to participants. We consider how and when gaze shifts may modulate person perception and its associated behavioral products. 相似文献
54.
55.
56.
57.
58.
59.
John Neil Martin 《Synthese》2008,165(1):31-51
Though acknowledged by scholars, Plato’s identification of the Beautiful and the Good has generated little interest, even
in aesthetics where the moral concepts are a current topic. The view is suspect because, e.g., it is easy to find examples
of ugly saints and beautiful sinners. In this paper the thesis is defended using ideas from Plato’s ancient commentators,
the Neoplatonists. Most interesting is Proclus, who applied to value theory a battery of linguistic tools with fixed semantic
properties—comparative adjectives, associated gradable adjectives, mass nouns, and predicate negations—all with a semantics
that demand a privative scale of value. It is shown how it is perfectly possible to interpret value terms Platonically over
privative Boolean algebras so that beautifuland good diverge while at higher levels other value terms are coextensional. Considerations are offered that this structure conforms
to actual usage. 相似文献
60.
Reginald?B.?AdamsJr.Email author Nalini?Ambady C.?Neil?Macrae Robert?E.?Kleck 《Motivation and emotion》2006,30(2):177-186
The contention that basic behavioral intentions are forecasted by emotional expressions has received surprisingly little empirical
support. We introduce a behavioral task that gauges the speed with which movement of angry and fearful faces (toward or away
from an expressor's gaze) are accurately detected. In two studies we found that perceivers were faster to correctly detect
approaching anger faces (i.e., faces that moved in the direction of their own gaze). The opposite, however, was not true for
fear expressions. These findings offer evidence that, at least for anger displays, the basic behavioral intent to approach
is strongly transmitted and at very low-levels of processing, even priming congruent behavioral responses in observers. The
null results for fear faces may indicate that these signal a “freezing” response or behavioral inhibition rather than flight
per se. The results of this work are discussed in relation to contemporary theories of emotion. 相似文献