全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1747篇 |
免费 | 32篇 |
专业分类
1779篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 14篇 |
2020年 | 21篇 |
2019年 | 17篇 |
2018年 | 26篇 |
2017年 | 45篇 |
2016年 | 33篇 |
2015年 | 34篇 |
2014年 | 26篇 |
2013年 | 238篇 |
2012年 | 46篇 |
2011年 | 55篇 |
2010年 | 34篇 |
2009年 | 36篇 |
2008年 | 46篇 |
2007年 | 60篇 |
2006年 | 56篇 |
2005年 | 46篇 |
2004年 | 37篇 |
2003年 | 46篇 |
2002年 | 54篇 |
2001年 | 25篇 |
2000年 | 20篇 |
1999年 | 21篇 |
1998年 | 20篇 |
1997年 | 16篇 |
1996年 | 20篇 |
1995年 | 15篇 |
1994年 | 20篇 |
1993年 | 16篇 |
1992年 | 27篇 |
1991年 | 30篇 |
1990年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 15篇 |
1987年 | 19篇 |
1986年 | 26篇 |
1985年 | 25篇 |
1984年 | 29篇 |
1983年 | 26篇 |
1982年 | 34篇 |
1981年 | 22篇 |
1980年 | 27篇 |
1979年 | 24篇 |
1978年 | 25篇 |
1977年 | 24篇 |
1976年 | 23篇 |
1975年 | 19篇 |
1974年 | 31篇 |
1973年 | 23篇 |
1971年 | 16篇 |
1970年 | 17篇 |
排序方式: 共有1779条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
171.
Episodic memory and the hippocampus: it's about time 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Several recent studies have sought to develop animal models of episodic memory, the capacity to recollect unique personal experiences. However, these studies have not yet provided unambiguous evidence that this capacity is based on recollection of the learning episodes. A recent study that examined memory for the ordering of events within unique experiences, and demonstrated a critical and selective role for the hippocampus, suggests a new and promising model for neurobiological analyses of episodic memory. 相似文献
172.
T.John Rosen Nathaniel S Terry Howard Leventhal 《Journal of research in personality》1982,16(1):90-107
Two experiments examined the relations among self-esteem, perceived competency to cope, and actual coping behaviors following a threat communication. Leventhal's “parallel response model” (in Advances in experimental social psychology, L. Berkowitz (Ed.), New York: Academic Press, 1970, Vol. 5) predicts that low-esteem subjects will show deficits in both competency and coping behaviors. Experiment 1 manipulated threat level of a tetanus communication. Low-esteem subjects showed coping deficits on measures of free associations, free recall, fatalism, and coping. Threat groups differed only on fear and danger measures. Experiment 2 manipulated the fear level of an antismoking film and used false feedback to alter perceived competency. Positive feedback increased perceived competency to quit smoking among low-esteem subjects only. Without feedback, low-esteem subjects reduced smoking less than high-esteem subjects; positive feedback reversed the pattern. The discussion argued that, consistent with Leventhal's model, the low-esteem coping deficit has two independent causes: (1) excessive concern with fear, and (2) inadequate perceived competency. 相似文献
173.
Jordan Howard Sobel 《Philosophical Studies》1992,67(1):51-69
To resolve putative liar paradoxes it is sufficient to attend to the distinction between liar-sentences and the propositions they would express, and to exercise the option of turning would-be deductions of paradox (of contradictions) into reductios of the existence of those propositions. Defending the coherence of particular resolutions along these lines, leads to recognition of the non-extensionality of some liarsentences. In particular, it turns out that exchanges of terms for identicals in the open-sentence — does not expression a true proposition are not invariably truth-preserving because they are not invariably proposition-expression preserving. All of this recommends propositions as fruitful subjects of interesting renewed research. 相似文献
174.
J H Howard S A Mutter D V Howard 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》1992,18(5):1029-1039
Serial pattern learning was investigated in a variation of the task introduced by Nissen and Bullemer (1987). We presented an asterisk at 1 of 4 spatial locations on each trial, and Ss either responded with a keypress or observed the event. The first 4 blocks contained 10 repetitions of a 10- or 16-element pattern, and the 5th block contained a random sequence. The difference in response time on the 5th random block and the previous patterned block served as an indirect measure of pattern learning. A direct measure was obtained in a final test block in which Ss predicted the next asterisk position. Equivalent learning occurred for responding and observing with indirect measures, but observation was superior with direct measures. These findings indicate that knowledge of serial order can develop through simple perceptual experience, and this is more available to deliberate recall than is knowledge acquired while responding. 相似文献
175.
Eighty-two women, presenting as normal-weight bulimics, obese binge eaters, social phobics, and individuals with panic disorder, were compared on anxiety, depression, and substance abuse. All were administered the Anxiety Disorder Interview Schedule-Revised and completed the Michigan Alcohol Screening Test, Drug Abuse Screening Test, and Self-Consciousness Scale. A striking proportion of eating disorder subjects were comorbid for one or more anxiety disorders, the most frequent diagnoses being generalized anxiety disorder and social phobia. The results suggest that the place of anxiety in bulimia nervosa goes beyond that discussed within the context of the anxiety reduction model. Conflicting comorbidity findings among this and prior investigations are noted, however, and discussed in terms of the issue of differential diagnosis between eating and anxiety disorders. 相似文献
176.
Tests of central auditory function and measures of receptive and expressive language abilities were administered to 10 children suspected of having central auditory processing disorders. Eight of the 10 subjects displayed expressive language difficulties, suggesting a need to examine more closely the relationship between expressive language and central auditory processing. 相似文献
177.
Adult age differences in the rate of learning serial patterns: evidence from direct and indirect tests. 总被引:9,自引:0,他引:9
Subjects performed a serial reaction time task (adopted from Nissen & Bullemer, 1987) that contained a repeating pattern of spatial locations. In Experiment 1, following 20 repetitions of a 10- or 16-element pattern, reaction time was equally disrupted for both younger and older people when the sequence became random. In Experiment 2, the response times for subjects encountering the 10-element pattern were compared with those of subjects encountering a random sequence. These response time functions diverged at the same point in training for the 2 age groups. Thus, on this indirect measure of response time facilitation, both experiments revealed age similarity in the rate of pattern learning. In contrast, on a subsequent direct test of pattern learning that required prediction, the younger people earned a higher percentage correct score than the older in both experiments. Age-related dissociations between direct and indirect measures of learning and comparisons with memory-impaired populations are discussed. 相似文献
178.
Daniel D. Houlihan Robert N. Jones Howard N. Sloane Joni Cook 《Behavioral Interventions》1992,7(1):71-77
This brief report proposes an extension of the traditional Alternating Treatments Design that would be acceptable to use in hospital and residential settings where subjects are often not concurrently available. Concerns about the inability of traditional designs to work in these special situations and the emergence of a “scientist–practitioner split” are discussed. An example of how the Extended Alternating Treatments Design might be used is provided. 相似文献
179.
Diane M. Sainato Howard Goldstein Phillip S. Strain 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1992,25(1):127-141
This study investigated effects of a self-evaluation procedure on preschool children's use of social interaction strategies among their classmates with autism. Three triads of children (comprised of 1 trained normally developing peer, 1 untrained peer, and 1 child with autism) participated. A multiple baseline design across subjects was used to demonstrate that peers who were taught facilitative strategies increased their use of strategies only after the self-evaluation intervention was introduced. Improvements in social behavior of children with autism was associated with peers' increased strategy use. Untrained peers demonstrated little change in their social behavior. Treatment effects were replicated when trained peers were asked to use self-evaluation with other children with autism during other play times. Self-evaluation procedures enhanced the use of social interaction strategies on the part of normally developing peers during social skills interventions. 相似文献
180.
A 39-year-old female suffered bitemporal infarctions and exhibited an unusual behavioral syndrome of muteness and impaired auditory comprehension, with milder aphasic deficits in reading and writing. The anatomic basis of this modality-selective communication disorder appeared to involve both peripheral perceptual and motor as well as “central” language mechanisms. The patient's ability to communicate was greatly aided by instruction in Amerind and Ameslan Sign Language, in which she learned more than 100 signs. Patients whose acquired language deficits spare modalities of input or output may be ideal candidates for sign language therapy. 相似文献