全文获取类型
收费全文 | 313篇 |
免费 | 30篇 |
专业分类
343篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 10篇 |
2019年 | 11篇 |
2018年 | 12篇 |
2017年 | 13篇 |
2016年 | 21篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 11篇 |
2013年 | 34篇 |
2012年 | 25篇 |
2011年 | 17篇 |
2010年 | 10篇 |
2009年 | 12篇 |
2008年 | 13篇 |
2007年 | 11篇 |
2006年 | 5篇 |
2005年 | 8篇 |
2004年 | 8篇 |
2003年 | 11篇 |
2002年 | 8篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 6篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
1967年 | 3篇 |
1963年 | 2篇 |
1962年 | 2篇 |
1960年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有343条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
Trait forgivingness is the disposition to forgive interpersonal transgressions over time and across situations. We define forgiveness as the replacement of negative unforgiving emotions with positive, other-oriented emotions. Rumination has been suggested as a mediator between forgivingness and emotional outcomes; however, we suggest that different content of rumination leads to different outcomes after transgressions. In four studies of 179, 233, 80, and 66 undergraduate students, trait forgivingness was negatively correlated with trait anger, hostility, neuroticism, fear, and vengeful rumination and was positively correlated with agreeableness, extraversion, and trait empathy. The disposition to ruminate vengefully mediated the relationship between trait forgivingness and (1) anger-related traits and (2) both revenge motivations and state anger following a specific recent transgression, but it did not mediate between forgivingness and (1) fearfulness and (2) avoidance motivations following a specific transgression. Self-hate statements, a proxy for depressive rumination, mediated the relationship between forgivingness and both depression and fearfulness but not the relationship between forgivingness and trait anger. Future research should distinguish the contents of mental rumination following interpersonal transgressions. 相似文献
82.
Nathaniel J. Pallone James J. Hennessy 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》1998,17(2-3):197-209
On the basis largely of interview data, this article outlines a working model for a process psychology paradigm for the heroic rescue fantasy and contrasts a prototypi-cal sequence therefore with corresponding sequences in antisocial, asocial, and prosocial risk-taking. It proposes that the heroic rescue fantasy emerges from the interplay between (a) neurologic anomaly affecting realistic assessment of costs, risks, and benefits associated with behavior of various sorts, (b) perpetuating as impulsivity expressed as persistent underestimation of costs and risks and perhaps supported by a condition of alexithymia or verbal deficit in cognitive functioning, interacting with (c) a persistent need for self-aggrandizement likely born of psychosocial developmental experiences, leading to (d) sensitization to interpersonal environments that provide role models for and positively reinforce prosocial risk-taking behavior, yielding to (e) the manipulative creation of an opportunity to behave in such fashion as to engender risk to others and to the self, and eventuating (f) in the enactment of a heroic rescue that prevents harm to others and thus elicits praise, admiration, adulation, and possibly formal reward. The conjunction between impulsivity expressing itself in the underesti-mation of costs (neurogenic or not) and need for self-aggrandizement is proposed as necessary in the heroic rescue fantasy, the manipulative creation of an opportunity to behave that engenders risk to others and to the self is proposed as the defining behavior, and a rescue that prevents harm to others is proposed as the defining crite-rion. 相似文献
83.
Different Training Procedures Recruit Either One or Two Critical Periods for Contextual Memory Consolidation, Each of Which Requires Protein Synthesis and PKA 总被引:11,自引:5,他引:11 下载免费PDF全文
Roussoudan Bourtchouladze Ted Abel Nathaniel Berman Rachael Gordon Kyle Lapidus Eric R. Kandel 《Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.)》1998,5(4):365-374
We have used a combined genetic and pharmacological approach to define the time course of the requirement for protein kinase A (PKA) and protein synthesis in long-term memory for contextual fear conditioning in mice. The time course of amnesia in transgenic mice that express R(AB) and have genetically reduced PKA activity in the hippocampus parallels that observed both in mice treated with inhibitors of PKA and mice treated with inhibitors of protein synthesis. This PKA- and protein synthesis-dependent memory develops between 1 hr and 3 hr after training. By injecting the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin or the PKA inhibitor Rp-cAMPs at various times after training, we find that depending on the nature of training, contextual memory has either one or two brief consolidation periods requiring synthesis of new proteins, and each of these also requires PKA. Weak training shows two time periods of sensitivity to inhibitors of protein synthesis and PKA, whereas stronger training exhibits only one. These studies underscore the parallel dependence of long-term contextual memory on protein synthesis and PKA and suggest that different training protocols may recruit a common signaling pathway in distinct ways. 相似文献
84.
Restraint and Cancellation: Multiple Inhibition Deficits in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
Schachar R Logan GD Robaey P Chen S Ickowicz A Barr C 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2007,35(2):229-238
We used variations of the stop signal task to study two components of motor response inhibition—the ability to withhold a
strong response tendency (restraint) and the ability to cancel an ongoing action (cancellation)—in children with a diagnosis
of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and in non-ADHD controls of similar age (ages 7–14 years). The goal was
to determine if restraint and cancellation were related and if both were deficient in ADHD. The stop signal task involved
a choice reaction time task (go task) which required a rapid response. The demand for inhibitory control was invoked through
the presentation of a stop signal on a subset of go trials which required that the ongoing response be suspended. The stop
signal was presented either concurrently with the go signal (restraint version) or after a variable delay (cancellation version).
In Study 1, we compared ADHD and control children on the cancellation version of the stop task; in Study 2, we compared ADHD
and controls on the restraint version. In Study 3, a subset of ADHD and control participants completed both tasks so that
we could examine convergence of these dimensions of inhibition. Compared to control participants, ADHD participants showed
a deficit both in the ability to cancel and to restrain a speeded motor response. Performance on the restraint version was
significantly correlated with performance on the cancellation version in controls, but not in ADHD participants. We conclude
that ADHD is associated with deficits in both restraint and cancellation subcomponents of inhibition. 相似文献
85.
Andrew J. Seidman Nathaniel G. Wade David L. Vogel Patrick Armistead-Jehle 《Military psychology》2019,31(1):11-17
This study assessed the relationship between the stigma of seeking psychological help and use of outpatient behavioral health services over a 2-year period among active duty military service members initially referred for neuropsychological evaluation secondary to their histories of mild traumatic brain injury. Although research has examined how stigma predicts proxies for help-seeking (i.e., attitudes towards/intentions to use services), very little research has looked at actual behavior, and studies that do have largely focused on previous use. In this study, we examined the relationship between participants’ stigma and subsequent behavioral health use. Our results indicated that whereas greater self-stigma (i.e., negative self-judgments for seeking psychological help) was associated with attending fewer behavioral health care sessions, public stigma (i.e., perceptions of public attitudes towards people who seek psychological help) was not associated with service use. These findings support the need for addressing the self-stigma associated with seeking behavioral health care. 相似文献
86.
87.
On the origin and preservation of cumulative record in its struggle for life as a favored term 下载免费PDF全文
This paper offers a case study of the origins, emergence, and evolution of the term cumulative record as the name for the means by which B. F. Skinner brought his behavior under the control of his subject matter. Our methods included on-line searches, reviews of Skinner's publications, and journal codings and counts. The results reveal that the term is not originally attributable to Skinner, but emerged earlier in ordinary language and in another discipline--education. It was not even original to Skinner in print in his own science. Still, the term was once original to him, which we address with additional analyses of his having originated and advanced it. We conclude with a discussion the constraints of our methods, suggestions for future research, and the variable appreciation of technology and terminology in science studies. 相似文献
88.
Allain P Nicoleau S Pinon K Etcharry-Bouyx F Barré J Berrut G Dubas F Le Gall D 《Brain and cognition》2005,57(1):4-7
A particularly important aspect of executive functioning involves the ability to form and carry out complex plans, that is to say planning. This study aimed to investigate planning in 18 older and 16 younger normal participants using an ecological planning subtask derived from the Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome test battery, the "Zoo Map Test." There are two trials. The first trial consists of a "high demand" version of the subtask in which the participants must plan in advance the order in which they will visit designated locations in a zoo (formulation level). In the second, or "low demand" version, the participant is simply required to follow a concrete externally imposed strategy to reach the locations to visit (execution level). The two-way ANOVAs mainly showed more difficulties in elderly adults than in younger adults, more difficulties in formulation level than in execution level, and lastly a greater difference between formulation and execution in older participants than in younger adults. These results suggest that elderly participants have some problems developing logical strategies whereas they are able to execute complex predetermined plans. 相似文献
89.
Three experiments were conducted to explore the effects of methylphenidate (MPH), attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) diagnosis, and age on performance on a complex visual-memory search task. Results showed that the effects of MPH varied with information load. On low-processing loads, all doses of MPH helped children with ADHD to improve accuracy with no cost to reaction time (RT), whereas on high loads, higher MPH doses improved error rates while slowing RT. Without medication, children with ADHD showed high error rates and slow RTs across both low and high loads, as did younger, normal control children. Because MPH slowed performance on only the most difficult, high-load conditions, it is argued that the drug improves self-regulatory ability, enabling children with ADHD to adapt differentially to high and low loads. 相似文献
90.
This study explored relationships between “highly probable” neuropathology in the limbic system (as gauged by clinically elevated
scores on the Limbic System Check-list-33, or LSCL-33, a screening index for limbic system dysfunction developed by medical
researchers at Harvard and Dartmouth) and inventoried psychopathology (as gauged through scores on the MMPI) among a group
of adjudicated pedophiles incarcerated for treatment in a specialized prison for criminal sexual psychopaths in a mid-Atlantic
state. Principal findings: Only 33 percent of this sample of incarcerated pedophiles exhibits scores on the LSCL-33 at a level high enough to warrant
a medical conclusion of “probable” limbic system dysfunction. When scores on the MMPI are grouped according to critical values
for clinically relevant scores on the LSCL-33, significant F ratios in one-way analyses of variance are observed on L, F,
K, Hs, D, Hy, Pa, Pt, Sc, and Ma. Mean T scores for “probable” dysfunctionals simultaneously reach elevations at or above
the threshold of clinical significance for Pd, Pa, and Sc. On that basis, the categorization psychopathic paranoid schizophrenia readily applies to limbic system dysfunctional child sexual abusers studied in this inquiry, but not to their nondysfunctional
counterparts. But significant F ratios are observed neither for the age of the victim(s) in the instant offense(s), for the
number of prior arrests for sex offenses, for the number of prior arrests for any and all felony offenses, including sex offenses,
nor for the offender's current age. However, a stepwise multiple regression between gross number of prior arrests for any
and all offenses (including sex offenses) as the most robust of the indices of past criminal activity and both LSCL-33 and
MMPI scores across all offenders represented in this inquiry (i.e., the limbic system dysfunctional, the nondysfunctional,
and a mid-range group) proceeds to four steps, achieving a multiple R of .575 and encompassing Pd, Mf, Pa, and LSCL-33 scores
as the most efficient interactive post-dictive set. Stepwise multiple regression analyses of past arrest record computed independently
for limbic system dysfunctionals and nondysfunctionals progresses among dysfunctionals to seven steps, achieving a multiple
R of .867 and encompassing Pd, Hy, Ma, Mf, Si, Pa, and Sc scores as the most efficient interactive post-dictive set; and among
nondysfunctionals, to six steps, achieving a multiple R of .797 and encompassing Mf, D, Pt, Hy, Pa, and over-controlled hostility
scores as the most efficient interactive post-dictive set. The latter results are interpreted as suggesting that neuropathology
and psychopathology interact with and potentiate each other in post-dicting criminal behavior among the pedophiles studied
in this inquiry.
This study was supported in part by the Fund for Correctional and Forensic Psychology, Rutgers University Foundation. The
investigators express deep appreciation to Charles Albino for assis-tance in data collection. 相似文献