全文获取类型
收费全文 | 960篇 |
免费 | 27篇 |
专业分类
987篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 14篇 |
2022年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 11篇 |
2020年 | 37篇 |
2019年 | 26篇 |
2018年 | 36篇 |
2017年 | 37篇 |
2016年 | 54篇 |
2015年 | 33篇 |
2014年 | 56篇 |
2013年 | 99篇 |
2012年 | 64篇 |
2011年 | 63篇 |
2010年 | 33篇 |
2009年 | 28篇 |
2008年 | 30篇 |
2007年 | 50篇 |
2006年 | 33篇 |
2005年 | 18篇 |
2004年 | 25篇 |
2003年 | 20篇 |
2002年 | 14篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1977年 | 8篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1970年 | 4篇 |
1967年 | 3篇 |
1966年 | 3篇 |
1962年 | 3篇 |
1959年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有987条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
Nathan Brody 《Intelligence》2009,37(4):429-430
22.
Nathan Ryan Nina Westera Mark Kebbell Rebecca Milne Mark Harrison 《Applied cognitive psychology》2020,34(3):565-576
Missing body homicide cases have gained public interest globally due to some high-profile cases. In many of these cases, the task of locating the victim's remains relies on the information investigators can gain through the interviewing of willing suspects. To date, investigative interviewing research has largely focused on the retrieval of episodic memory (events) without focusing on spatial memory, a prominent cognitive task required in locating a victim's remains. The current experiment tests the enhanced cognitive interview (ECI) against a free recall strategy in a mock homicide scenario where participants are required to hide and retrieve an object in a natural bushland setting. The results showed that those in the ECI condition produced more coarse- and fine-grained details of landmarks and their actions at and journeying to the deposition site. This demonstrates the value of using the ECI in generating more valuable information to assist in successive search attempts. 相似文献
23.
Carmen E. Westerberg Nathan Wofford Safia Menssor Bryant P. Reininger Rebecca G. Deason 《Journal of Cognitive Psychology》2020,32(7):587-597
ABSTRACT Faces belonging to the same category as the perceiver are better recognised than faces from different categories when tested immediately. After a delay, the same-category benefit persists and response bias becomes more liberal for other-category but not same-category faces. These effects are typically attributed to better encoding of same-category than other-category faces. To determine whether bias effects after a delay persist when immediate accuracy differences are minimised, Hispanic and Caucasian participants were given more study time for other-category than same-category faces. Memory was tested immediately and after a two-day delay. Discrimination accuracy was higher for other-category than for same-category faces during both test sessions. Bias was equivalent immediately, but after the delay, bias was more conservative for same-category than for other-category faces, especially in Caucasian participants. These results suggest that post-encoding processes differentially influence face memories from different categories, which may have implications for evaluating face memory in everyday situations. 相似文献
24.
25.
26.
It is now more than 50 years that the Goodman paradox has been discussed, and many different solutions have been proposed. But so far no agreement has been reached about which is the correct solution to the paradox. In this paper, I present the naturalistic solutions to the paradox that were proposed in Quine (1969, 1974), Quine and Ullian (1970/1978), and Stemmer (1971). At the same time, I introduce a number of modifications and improvements that are needed for overcoming shortcomings of the solutions. The discussion of this improved version suggests that the Goodman paradox actually embodies three different problems; yet, one of them is not Goodman’s but Hume’s problem. The discussion also suggests that the naturalistic approach is probably the best for basing on it a theory of confirmation. Finally, I analyze one of Hume’s insights that seems to have been largely ignored. This insight shows a surprising similarity to a central feature of the naturalistic solutions. 相似文献
27.
In this cross-sectional study, the authors examined the relationship between emotion and behavior regulation in 3- and 5-year olds. Eighty-seven children performed a compliance sorting task. The authors manipulated the demand for emotion regulation by presenting and then hiding toys (low) or making toys visible (high). Mothers and teachers rated children's coping responses. Five-year-olds sorted less in the high condition than in the low condition, and 3-year-olds spent equal time sorting in both conditions. Compliance was positively correlated with problem-focused coping and negatively correlated with emotion-focused coping. Correlations between emotion and behavior regulation were stronger for the 5-year-olds. Age groups were differently affected by the higher demands of emotion regulation, indicating that the child's resources for regulation interact with the task demands to determine behavioral outcome. 相似文献
28.
Nathan Steven Carlin 《Journal of religion and health》2003,42(3):231-249
The present study is a psychoanalytic reading of Archpriest Avvakum's autobiography and takes its cue from some observations made by Russian and Ukrainian historian Edward Keenan, who offered a tentative diagnosis of manic-depression to describe Avvakum's personality. In my view, Donald Capps's analysis of religious male melancholia supports Keenan's observations, and I argue that Avvakum carried his childhood experiences and conflicts over into his later religious life. His religious life manifested a series of transferences and displacements, with Mary the Mother of God, God the Father, and the Church functioning as his loving or positive relationships with his family; and Patriarch Nikon and his followers embodying everything he feared and resented about his own childhood—change and abandonment. 相似文献
29.
30.
In this essay we argue that infant temperament, defined as behavioral styles that appear early in life as a direct result of neurobiological factors, plays a significant role in the development and expression of social behavior. Temperament may be studied using a typological or dimensional approach and the relations between early temperament and later behavior can be examined in terms of homotypic or heterotypic continuity. The implication of each of these approaches for understanding the influence of infant temperament on social development is examined. As well, utilizing data from our laboratory, we suggest that under certain circumstances one can predict social behavior by identifying two temperament types in infancy. These types are high reactive/high negative infants and high reactive/high positive infants. A significant proportion of the high reactive/high negative infants display behavioral inhibition and social reticence through the preschool years, whereas a significant proportion of the high reactive/high positive infants display exuberance and positive social interaction through the preschool period. 相似文献