全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1671篇 |
免费 | 59篇 |
专业分类
1730篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 11篇 |
2022年 | 8篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 20篇 |
2019年 | 21篇 |
2018年 | 31篇 |
2017年 | 36篇 |
2016年 | 42篇 |
2015年 | 44篇 |
2014年 | 34篇 |
2013年 | 161篇 |
2012年 | 61篇 |
2011年 | 65篇 |
2010年 | 45篇 |
2009年 | 48篇 |
2008年 | 70篇 |
2007年 | 60篇 |
2006年 | 69篇 |
2005年 | 52篇 |
2004年 | 58篇 |
2003年 | 66篇 |
2002年 | 65篇 |
2001年 | 23篇 |
2000年 | 22篇 |
1999年 | 29篇 |
1998年 | 38篇 |
1997年 | 38篇 |
1996年 | 25篇 |
1995年 | 31篇 |
1994年 | 32篇 |
1993年 | 35篇 |
1992年 | 33篇 |
1991年 | 23篇 |
1990年 | 25篇 |
1989年 | 20篇 |
1988年 | 16篇 |
1987年 | 21篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 21篇 |
1984年 | 15篇 |
1983年 | 21篇 |
1982年 | 15篇 |
1981年 | 25篇 |
1980年 | 21篇 |
1979年 | 24篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 12篇 |
1976年 | 20篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有1730条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
11.
Locational representation in imagery: The third dimension 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Nancy H. Kerr 《Memory & cognition》1987,15(6):521-530
Six experiments were conducted to test the relative processing characteristics of picture-plane and three-dimensional imagery as indexed by tasks that required subjects to keep track of successive locations in multiunit visual displays. Subjects were shown symmetrical displays either drawn on cardboard or constructed with three-dimensional blocks. They then were required to imagine these matrices and follow pathways through a series of adjacent squares (blocks) within the matrices. The pathways were described by a series of verbal terms that indicated the direction of the next square (block) in the pathway. Subjects experienced difficulty in performing the task with picture-plane displays composed of as few as 16 squares (4×4), but they rarely made errors with a three-dimensional matrix of 27 blocks (3×3×3). Performance with the three-dimensional task dropped dramatically when the matrix size was increased to 4×4×4. The results replicated previous findings that the image processing capacity for location in two-dimensional imagery is about three units in each direction, and they indicate that adding the depth dimension increases the capacity for representation of spatial location in imagery. 相似文献
12.
13.
Margaret L. Signorella Martha Hansen Krupa Wesley Jamison Nancy Lyons 《Sex roles》1986,14(9-10):475-479
Newcombe, Bandura, and Taylor developed a questionnaire measuring participation in space-related activities. The activities were classified as masculine, feminine, or neutral in gender stereotyping. A short version of this questionnaire was developed based on an item analysis of the responses by 485 introductory psychology students (236 women and 249 men). The 10 masculine, 10 feminine, and 10 neutral items chosen for the short form were given to 60 subjects (28 women and 32 men), along with a measure of spatial performance (Piaget's water-level task). Reliabilities (by coefficient alpha) were .79, .77, and .75, for the masculine, feminine, and neutral activity subscales, respectively. As with the original scale, women had a greater preference than men for feminine activities, men had a greater preference than women for masculine activities, and women and men had an equal preference for neutral activities. Masculine activity preference was associated with better water-level performance, with the relationship significantly stronger for women (r=.50) than for men (r=.10). Uses for the questionnaire in research on sex-related differences in spatial performance are suggested. 相似文献
14.
Nancy L. Etcoff 《Brain and cognition》1984,3(4):385-412
Patients with lesions to either the right or left hemisphere and control subjects were asked to judge the similarity of pairs of photographs of a person displaying different emotions, and of pairs of emotion words. The results were submitted to a multidimensional scaling analysis. Right-hemisphere-damaged subjects were found to be more impaired at perceiving facial emotions than were left-hemisphere-damaged subjects or controls, and this impairment was not confined to the perception of a subset of facial emotions nor to judging emotional valence (Pleasantness versus Unpleasantness). Rather, subtle impairments in perceiving a wide range of facial emotions were found, mostly concerning differentiation of the Positive-Negative and Attention-Rejection dimensions, and concerning the strategies the subjects used to make their judgments. The right-hemisphere-damaged subjects performed comparably to controls in their ratings of emotion words, suggesting that their ability to conceptualize emotional states was intact and that their impairment was strictly in the perception of emotion. 相似文献
15.
R.Edward Geiselman Nancy A Haight Lori G Kimata 《Journal of experimental social psychology》1984,20(5):409-424
Four experiments were conducted to study the nature of context effects on the perceived physical attractiveness of faces. In Experiment 1, photos of faces scaled on attractiveness were presented in sets of three, with target faces appearing in the middle flanked by two context faces. The target faces were of average attractiveness, with the context faces being either high, average, or low in attractiveness. The effect of the context was one of assimilation, rather than contrast, regardless of whether the persons in the photos were portrayed to be associated. This result was interpreted in terms of a “generalized halo effect” for judgments of the physical attractiveness of stimuli within a group. Presenting the persons of a set as friends enhanced the perceived attractiveness of the target face but only when the context did not contain a face of low attractiveness. In Experiment 2, the assimilation effect was observed to carry over to influence ratings of the target faces several minutes after the context faces had been removed. Experiment 3 showed the assimilation effect to be robust regardless of whether the context was composed of two faces or one, but Experiment 4 showed the assimilation effect to be evident only when the context faces were presented simultaneously with the target. 相似文献
16.
R. Lillianne Macias Nancy Nava Desiree Delgadillo Jordyn Beschel Gabriel Kuperminc 《American journal of community psychology》2023,71(1-2):114-122
In this article, we share findings from a qualitative case study of a virtual youth photovoice program implemented across three regions of the United States. The purpose of the program was to engage youth in research on a social issue relevant to them during an unprecedented year marked by two public health crises, COVID-19 and anti-Black racial violence. Results of an analysis of curriculum and archival program materials lend support for online strategies for youth engagement including individualized support and online audiovisual presentations with avatars. Racial justice and trauma-informed adaptations were designed to be responsive to youth needs for flexible programming and safe spaces. Themes captured in the first online gallery of youth photos include (1) tools for mental health, (2) meaningful connection, and (3) community advocacy, bringing attention to structural issues as well as family and community strengths. Findings suggest photovoice can be thoughtfully adapted for youth researchers and support individual and group storytelling in response to collective trauma. 相似文献
17.
Animal Cognition - Since ecology influences the expression of cognitive traits, intra-specific variation in ecological demands can drive differences in cognition. This is often the case, for... 相似文献
18.
Ronald C. Martella Ph.D. Ian J. Leonard B.S. Nancy E. Marchand-Martella Ph.D. Martin Agran Ph.D. 《Journal of Behavioral Education》1993,3(1):77-86
We examined the effects of a self-monitoring program on reducing the frequency of negative statements of a student with mild mental retardation who was enrolled in a self-contained classroom at a middle school. A changing criterion design was used within a multiple baseline design across two classroom periods. Additionally, the frequency of positive statements was reported. The results showed a decrease in the number of negative statements, and an increase in the number of positive statements. Maintenance data collected up to 8 weeks after the program ended indicated that the student continued to emit positive rather than negative statements. The use of this procedure to decelerate undesirable behavior is discussed. 相似文献
19.
David Foulkes Barbara Meier Inge Strauch Nancy H. Kerr Laura Bradley Michael Hollifield 《International journal of psychology》1993,28(6):871-891
Sixteen German-English bilinguals were studied in a sleep laboratory for four nonconsecutive nights each. Half were native English speakers living in Zürich, and half native German speakers living in Atlanta. Presleep thought samples were solicited each evening and REM dream reports each night; subjects judged the waking appropriateness of their imagined speech and language phenomena, and also identified waking sources of their dream imagery, the following mornings. Incidences of dreaming and of speech therein generally were similar to those of monolinguals. Whether sessions were conducted in German (two nights) or English (two nights) did directly influence language selection in subjects' dreams. Judged appropriateness of language selection to imagined events was very high for thought samples, and high for REM dreams. Sources for thought samples were generally consonant with the language dominant at study site; for REM dreams this relationship was considerably weaker. Judged waking appropriateness to imagined situations was more strongly related to language selection than was the language reference of the supposed sources of those situations. 相似文献
20.