全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1859篇 |
免费 | 20篇 |
专业分类
1879篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 14篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 21篇 |
2019年 | 24篇 |
2018年 | 36篇 |
2017年 | 40篇 |
2016年 | 47篇 |
2015年 | 47篇 |
2014年 | 46篇 |
2013年 | 178篇 |
2012年 | 64篇 |
2011年 | 79篇 |
2010年 | 52篇 |
2009年 | 54篇 |
2008年 | 80篇 |
2007年 | 67篇 |
2006年 | 76篇 |
2005年 | 59篇 |
2004年 | 62篇 |
2003年 | 70篇 |
2002年 | 67篇 |
2001年 | 23篇 |
2000年 | 28篇 |
1999年 | 31篇 |
1998年 | 38篇 |
1997年 | 38篇 |
1996年 | 26篇 |
1995年 | 32篇 |
1994年 | 31篇 |
1993年 | 35篇 |
1992年 | 33篇 |
1991年 | 24篇 |
1990年 | 25篇 |
1989年 | 20篇 |
1988年 | 16篇 |
1987年 | 23篇 |
1986年 | 18篇 |
1985年 | 23篇 |
1984年 | 15篇 |
1983年 | 21篇 |
1982年 | 15篇 |
1981年 | 26篇 |
1980年 | 24篇 |
1979年 | 26篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 12篇 |
1976年 | 20篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1973年 | 8篇 |
1971年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有1879条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
Nancy Frankenberry 《The Journal of religious ethics》1999,27(1):129-136
The philosophical question Nunez raises is whether we can have, as he thinks we need, a theoretical grounding for appeal to the intrinsic value of nature. This article examines the neopragmatist reasons for repudiating metaphysical realism's notions of intrinsicality and subject-independent reality. Following the holism of Donald Davidson and Richard Rorty rather than the epistemological premises of Holmes Rolston and Bernard Lonergan, the author concludes that coping with the ecological crisis does not require conjuring an epistemic crisis. Environmental ethics in neopragmatist hands would seek procedures for bringing about agreement in improving our practices, not our epistemology. 相似文献
82.
83.
Nancy Tuana 《Synthese》1995,104(3):441-461
This essay delineates the contributions of feminist critiques of science to contemporary reconstructions of empiricism. I argue that three central tenets arise from feminist attention to the dynamics of gender and oppression in the theories and methods of science: 1) a rejection of the science/politics dichotomy; 2) an acknowledgement of the epistemic import of subjective components of knowledge; and 3) a reconfiguration of the subject of knowledge. These three tenets are illustrated and supported through examples from the history of science. 相似文献
84.
In light of information uncovered about human radiation experiments conducted during the Cold War, an important charge for the Advisory Committee on Human Radiation Experiments was to assess the current state of protections for human research subjects. This assessment was designed to enhance the Committee's ability to make informed recommendations for the improvement of future policies and practices for the protection of research subjects. The Committee's examination of current protections revealed great improvement over those from the past, yet some problems remain. Although the data collected by the Committee highlight specific areas in need of attention, the Committee's work should be viewed in part as the beginning of a series of ongoing assessments of the adequacy and effectiveness of the protections afforded to human subjects. 相似文献
85.
Focusing on social pragmatics, this longitudinal study investigated the contribution of mother–toddler collaborative communication to theory of mind (ToM) development at age 4. At age 2½, 78 toddlers (42 boys) and their mothers were observed during pretend play. At age 4, children were tested using 4 false belief understanding tasks. Both mothers and toddlers engaged in more collaborative (inform, guide/request, and support/confirm) than non-collaborative communication acts. Other-focused collaborative acts of support/confirm by mothers and toddlers predicted children's false belief understanding, even after controlling for 5 covariates. In addition, as active agents in their own ToM development, the contribution of toddlers' collaborative acts to false belief understanding was independent of their mother's. Finally, the way toddlers and their mothers co-constructed their communication mattered. Only when toddlers engaged in high levels of collaborative acts, the mothers' high levels of collaborative acts demonstrated a positive effect on children's ToM development. The applied implications of these findings are discussed. 相似文献
86.
Chodorow NJ 《Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association》2003,51(4):1181-1198
This paper explores a particular constellation of fantasies and defenses that in some women leads to the delay of childbearing. Destructive wishes towards the woman's own womb and her mother's lead to behaviors that sabotage fertility and pregnancy. These wishes and behaviors meet up with an unconscious belief in and commitment to time standing still, and with cultural tropes and trends that obscure intrapsychic conflicts and denials about motherhood, aging, and time. Analysis, like childbearing, is constrained by the reality of time, and the complex untangling and connecting of these three not obviously related threads may not occur until it is, for the woman who wants a child, too late. 相似文献
87.
Some words have fewer direct associates than others, and, when words varying in set size are studied in a list-learning task, those with smaller sets are more likely to be recalled. This set-size effect is found in cued recall when the words are studied in the absence of related words, but not when studied in the presence of related words. Related words provide context and theoretically inhibit irrelevant associates. The present research determined that set-size effects are found when words are encoded in sentence contexts. In contrast to list-learning experiments, the results of three experiments found such effects even when lexically related words were present in the sentences. Other findings indicated that target-set-size effects were determined by the proximity of related words in the sentence and the nature of the test cue. The results are discussed in relation to a model for explaining set-size effects and to selective findings from the sentence-comprehension literature. 相似文献
88.
Switching points of view in spatial mental models 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
In six experiments, subjects read narratives describing varying spatial scenes with more than one point of view. They were probed with questions about objects located in six directions from each character's point of view. Subjects' response times were consistent with a one place-one perspective rule. They seemed to form separate mental models for separate places and to take a character's perspective when there was only one relevant character in a scene, but they seemed to take a neutral perspective when there was more than one probed point of view, rather than switch perspectives. 相似文献
89.
Nancy C. Higgins C. Alec Pollard William T. Merkel 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》1992,11(1):79-85
This exploratory study sought to determine whether selected religion-related factors differentiated between 86 patients with
obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD), 73 patients with panic disorder, and 292 patients with other psychiatric (non-anxiety)
disorders. A standard history questionnaire was used to obtain information from patients concerning religion of origin, involvement
in religious activities, religious conflict, and perceived religiousness of parents. It was found that the percentage of patients
who reported experiencing religious conflict was significantly higher for the OCD group than for the other two groups. Other
findings suggested associations between Catholicism and OCD and between Protestantism and panic disorder, but further research
is needed to clarify these relationships.
This article is based on a paper presented at the 98th annual convention of the American Psychological Association, Boston,
August 1990. 相似文献
90.
Joanna Blake Sheilah McConnell Gayle Horton Nancy Benson 《Infant and child development》1992,1(3):127-136
The entire repertoire of communicative gestures was documented in a longitudinal, observational study of 10 infants, whose combined ages covered the range from 9 to 22 months. Early in the second year, giving as a request to do something with the object increased, while emotive gestures decreased. Later in the second year, pointing gestures increased, while protest gestures tended to decrease. Combining gestures with vocalization tended to increase only for protest gestures later in the second year. Eye contact showed a small but continuous increase in coordination with gestures over the second year, particularly with comment, request, and emotive gestures. These findings indicate an increasing use of the parent as an agent and of the informative function in non-verbal communication during this period of transition to verbal communication. 相似文献