全文获取类型
收费全文 | 308篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
312篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 8篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 9篇 |
2019年 | 12篇 |
2018年 | 19篇 |
2017年 | 14篇 |
2016年 | 13篇 |
2015年 | 9篇 |
2014年 | 11篇 |
2013年 | 37篇 |
2012年 | 15篇 |
2011年 | 20篇 |
2010年 | 6篇 |
2009年 | 6篇 |
2008年 | 9篇 |
2007年 | 17篇 |
2006年 | 11篇 |
2005年 | 5篇 |
2004年 | 10篇 |
2003年 | 8篇 |
2002年 | 6篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 1篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有312条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
61.
Nadine Faulkner 《Philosophical Investigations》2010,33(2):159-183
In this paper I explore a neglected discussion of vagueness put forward by Wittgenstein in his Philosophical Grammar (1932–34). In this work, unlike Philosophical Investigations (1953), Wittgenstein not only discusses the venerable Sorites paradox but provides a novel conception of vagueness using an analogy with coin tossing and converging intervals. As he sees it, the problematic picture of vagueness arises because we conflate aspects of the functioning of vague concepts with those of non‐vague ones. Thus, while we accept that vague concepts have no sharp cut‐off points (are boundaryless), we nevertheless retain the idea that we can progress towards the penumbra the way we progress towards the cut‐off points of non‐vague concepts. As a potential remedy, Wittgenstein's analogy with coin tossing and converging intervals replaces this picture and provides an understanding of the functioning of vague concepts in which no notion of a progression arises. 相似文献
62.
Yasmin Ibrahim 《Contemporary Islam》2007,1(1):37-51
Since the bombing of the twin towers, ‘Islam’ as a cultural narrative has entered a new temporal phase which in many ways
signifies the re-imagining of a religion through the visual imagery of 9/11 and the global events which have unfolded since
the apocalyptic images were first witnessed by the world. The distant proximity of 9/11 and the relocation of the perceived
Islamic ‘resurgent atavism’ within the heartlands of power in Western soil constitute the formation of a new sociological
imagination of Islam and 9/11 as a new liminal temporality. The association of the religion with a new category of risks in
urban sites and the constant state of insecurity in seemingly secure spaces represents a new narrative phase of geo-politics
in which the locus of this re-imagining mediated through ICTs, is one that happens not just in faraway places but within the
‘ontological securities’ of Western modernity, posing a liquid threat which is impervious to territorially bounded spheres. 相似文献
63.
Hifumi Ohnishi Farah A. Ibrahim Steven V. Owen 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2001,20(3):250-259
This research describes an exploratory factor analysis for the purposes of factorial validation for both the English and Japanese
versions of the Objective Measure of Ego-Identity Status (OMEIS)—Ideological version (Adams, Bennion, & Huh, 1989). The Japanese
OMEIS is a direct translation of the English OMEIS (Ohnishi, 1998). The study reports the factor structure of the OMEIS—Ideological
version in English and Japanese. A principal factor analysis (PFA) with both oblique and orthogonal solutions was performed.
Alpha reliabilities for the factors are presented. The present research compares and contrasts the factor structures of OMEIS—Ideological
version, derived from a U.S. sample and a Japanese sample, comprising women from late adulthood to middle age. Cultural differences
between the two sample populations are explored. 相似文献
64.
Robert Ladouceur Claude Paquet Nadine Lachance Dominique Dub 《International journal of psychology》1996,31(2):93-99
Un des principaux facteurs expliquant le jeu excessif serait les perceptions erronées qu'entretiennent les individus concernant le hasard. Cet article vise à préciser les perceptions du joueur en analysant ses verbalisations lors de la production ou de la reconnaissance de suites aléatoires. Deux études empiriques ont été réalisées. E est prédit que la majorité des gens ont une perception erronée à l'égard du hasard et qu'il existe une relation entre le nombre d'issues possibles (par exemple deux lors du tir d'une pièce de monnaie ou six lors du tir d'un dé) et le nombre de verbalisations erronées faisant référence aux événements passés de la suite. Comme on le prévoyait, près de 70% des verbalisations sont l'expression d'une perception erronée, la plupart se référant à des liens de dépendance entre les événements de la suite. Cependant, l'augmentation du nombre d'issues possibles n'a pas d'effet significatif sur la proportion de perceptions erronées. La discussion met en relief les implications pratiques et théoriques des perceptions inadéquates des joueurs sur l'illusion du contrôle du jeu et des comportements excessifs en résultant. 相似文献
65.
Ibrahim M. Abu‐Rabi’ 《Islam & Christian-Muslim Relations》1997,8(1):85-97
The primary condition for the progress, or indeed the survival, of human societies is their effective coalescence into one human society’ (Zurayk 1994, 96). 相似文献
66.
Robert C. Beck Charles Gibson Wendy Elliott Carolyn Simmons Nadine Matteson Lisa McDaniel 《Motivation and emotion》1988,12(3):217-236
Four experiments examined the hypothesis that the Valins (1966) false physiological feedback effect with attractiveness ratings of slides is due to experimenter demand. Experiments 1 and 2 showed significant feedback effects with 5-sec feedback periods, previously reported by Barefoot and Straub (1971) to be too brief a time to search the slides for a cause of the apparent physiological arousal. Experiments 3 and 4 had 17 variations of instructions (emotional, nonemotional), stimuli (slides of people, scenic tourist slides), and type of feedback information (heart rate, eyeblink, or none). The typical false feedback effect was found under many conditions that did not seem to meet the presumptive attributional requirements for the effect. In Experiment 4, only subjects who said they were supposed to rate feedback slides higher showed the effect, regardless of instructions, stimuli, or type of feedback. The overall results are interpreted in terms of experimenter demand and stimulus salience effects. 相似文献
67.
68.
Carla Rice Eliza Chandler Jen Rinaldi Nadine Changfoot Kirsty Liddiard Roxanne Mykitiuk Ingrid Mündel 《希帕蒂亚:女权主义哲学杂志》2017,32(2):213-229
This article explores twelve short narrative films created by women and trans people living with disabilities and embodied differences. Produced through Project Re?Vision, these micro documentaries uncover the cultures and temporalities of bodies of difference by foregrounding themes of multiple histories: body, disability, maternal, medical, and/or scientific histories; and divergent futurities: contradictory, surprising, unpredictable, opaque, and/or generative futures. We engage with Alison Kafer's call to theorize disability futurity by wrestling with the ways in which “the future” is normatively deployed in the service of able‐bodiedness and able‐mindedness (Kafer 2013 ), a deployment used to render bodies of difference as sites of “no future” (Edelman 2004 ). By re‐storying embodied difference, the storytellers illuminate ongoing processes of remaking their bodily selves in ways that respond to the past and provide possibilities for different futures; these orientations may be configured as “dis‐topias” based not on progress, but on new pathways for living, uncovered not through evoking the familiar imaginaries of curing, eliminating, or overcoming disability, but through incorporating experiences of embodied difference into time. These temporalities gesture toward new kinds of futures, giving us glimpses of ways of cripping time, of cripping ways of being/becoming in time, and of radically re‐presencing disability in futurity. 相似文献
69.
Diego Sarracino Giancarlo Dimaggio Rawezh Ibrahim Raffaele Popolo Sandra Sassaroli Giovanni M. Ruggiero 《Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy》2017,35(3):278-295
ABC-DEF framework is at the core of rational emotive behavior therapy. It is a highly flexible framework and has proven to be applicable to many emotional disorders. We cannot take for granted, however, that this framework can be used successfully with all clients, particularly with those suffering from severe disorders or personality disorders. In fact, the difficulties of these clients in recognizing, naming and reflecting upon states of mind, their dysregulated emotions and self-defeating behavior, and their difficulty in establishing a strong working alliance with a therapist may hamper the correct implementation of the ABC-DEF framework and the disputing of their irrational beliefs. This paper aims to describe in detail the challenges that clients with personality disorders may pose during treatment and offer possible technical suggestions, derived from either REBT or non-REBT literature, that can help REBT and CBT practitioners adapting their interventions to resistant clients. 相似文献
70.