首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   19739篇
  免费   520篇
  国内免费   3篇
  2020年   139篇
  2019年   152篇
  2018年   1690篇
  2017年   1517篇
  2016年   1159篇
  2015年   208篇
  2014年   201篇
  2013年   808篇
  2012年   581篇
  2011年   1429篇
  2010年   1321篇
  2009年   791篇
  2008年   1085篇
  2007年   1321篇
  2006年   263篇
  2005年   302篇
  2004年   263篇
  2003年   220篇
  2002年   218篇
  2001年   428篇
  2000年   411篇
  1999年   271篇
  1998年   108篇
  1996年   96篇
  1995年   92篇
  1992年   208篇
  1991年   215篇
  1990年   206篇
  1989年   201篇
  1988年   205篇
  1987年   183篇
  1986年   172篇
  1985年   172篇
  1984年   158篇
  1983年   114篇
  1982年   114篇
  1979年   149篇
  1978年   94篇
  1977年   95篇
  1976年   92篇
  1975年   138篇
  1974年   157篇
  1973年   181篇
  1972年   155篇
  1971年   141篇
  1970年   124篇
  1969年   133篇
  1968年   175篇
  1967年   131篇
  1966年   141篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
12.
By roughly 6 years of age, children acquire the stereotype that men are more competent than women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM), potentially leading to greater trust in scientific information provided by men. This study tested whether 3- to 8-year-old children differentially endorsed conflicting information about science and toys presented by male and female informants depicted as a ‘man’ and ‘woman’ (Exp1) or ‘scientists’ (Exp2). Children were expected to endorse toy testimony from gender-matched informants; thus, the key question concerned endorsement of science testimony. In Exp1 (N = 149), boys and girls showed a same-gender informant preference for toy testimony; however, girls endorsed the male informant's testimony more for science than for toys – but only when tested by a male experimenter. In Exp2 (N = 264), boys and girls showed a same-gender preference, irrespective of content. Findings suggest that STEM-related gender stereotypes might lead girls to trust scientific information presented by men over women in certain contexts.  相似文献   
13.
14.
15.
Using both cross-sectional and longitudinal data spanning a decade, this article analyzes how dietary behavior of household children and adults (N = 576 households) was affected by the Stanford Five-City Project (FCP). Tests of a three-part, cumulative model of bidirectional influences within the family, which conceptualizes household members as sources of influence on each other and subject to influence by an external agent, were supported. Children and adults were influenced by both each other and the FCP campaign in changing and maintaining health behaviors. This article demonstrates that public health campaigns can be made more effective if they conceptualize both children and adults as potential sources of influence. The long-term effectiveness of such efforts can be enhanced by encouraging families, as opposed to individuals, to change health behaviors.  相似文献   
16.
17.
18.
19.
In Experiment 1, rats were trained on a discrimination between rubber- and sandpaper-covered arms of a maze after one group had been pre-exposed to these intra-maze cues. Pre-exposure facilitated subsequent discrimination learning, unless the discrimination was made easier by adding further discriminative stimuli, when it now significantly retarded learning. In Experiment 2, rats were trained on an extra-maze spatial discrimination, again after one group, but not another, had been pre-exposed to the extra-maze landmarks. Here too, pre-exposure facilitated subsequent discrimination learning, unless the discrimination was made substantially easier by arranging that the two arms between which rats had to choose were always separated by 135°. The results of both experiments can be explained by supposing that perceptual learning depends on the presence of features common to S+ and S-.  相似文献   
20.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号