全文获取类型
收费全文 | 32841篇 |
免费 | 693篇 |
国内免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
33538篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 329篇 |
2019年 | 407篇 |
2018年 | 606篇 |
2017年 | 607篇 |
2016年 | 645篇 |
2015年 | 493篇 |
2014年 | 540篇 |
2013年 | 2502篇 |
2012年 | 951篇 |
2011年 | 998篇 |
2010年 | 604篇 |
2009年 | 542篇 |
2008年 | 780篇 |
2007年 | 819篇 |
2006年 | 716篇 |
2005年 | 646篇 |
2004年 | 611篇 |
2003年 | 570篇 |
2002年 | 613篇 |
2001年 | 1110篇 |
2000年 | 1057篇 |
1999年 | 780篇 |
1998年 | 318篇 |
1996年 | 311篇 |
1992年 | 634篇 |
1991年 | 629篇 |
1990年 | 605篇 |
1989年 | 584篇 |
1988年 | 600篇 |
1987年 | 544篇 |
1986年 | 544篇 |
1985年 | 550篇 |
1984年 | 452篇 |
1983年 | 381篇 |
1982年 | 312篇 |
1981年 | 287篇 |
1979年 | 491篇 |
1978年 | 341篇 |
1977年 | 305篇 |
1976年 | 284篇 |
1975年 | 406篇 |
1974年 | 506篇 |
1973年 | 538篇 |
1972年 | 428篇 |
1971年 | 407篇 |
1970年 | 396篇 |
1969年 | 376篇 |
1968年 | 503篇 |
1967年 | 430篇 |
1966年 | 411篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
L B Johns 《Perceptual and motor skills》1989,68(2):359-364
This investigation explored the interaction of progressive-part versus whole methods of practice with hemispheric preference for processing information and the impact of each upon high school students' speed and accuracy in beginning typewriting. Zenhausern's Differential Hemispheric Activation Test was scored in such a way that it was possible to plot the scores along a continuum. Analysis of variance gave significant F ratios on 3 of the 4 testing days. The continuous scores were divided into five categories: middle, left moderates, right moderates, extreme rights, and extreme lefts. The moderate-left group speed was consistently the fastest group, and the extreme rights were consistently the slowest group. This difference was significant for all four testing days with the moderate-left mean speed varying between 4 to 6 words per minute faster each testing day. The extreme rights were consistently the most accurate, even though not statistically significantly so. There was no significant difference between method of practice and typewriting speed or between method of practice and typewriting accuracy; however, on all four testing days the mean gross speed of the whole practice learning group was 0.73 to 0.99 words per minute faster than the progressive-part group. A two-way analysis of variance indicated no interaction between method or practice and hemispheric preference. 相似文献
72.
73.
J B Murray 《Psychological reports》1989,64(2):627-644
Alcoholism among women often has been treated using a model based on research with alcoholic men. Women react differently to alcohol than men both physiologically and psychologically and their drinking patterns are different from men's. Treatment facilities for alcoholism have not been as available for women as for men and treatment must be individualized for the differences between men and women problem drinkers and for the differences among alcoholic women. 相似文献
74.
The naturalistic fallacy occurs when a person reads a report of scientific research and concludes that the moral implication of the research was included in the article when, in fact, it was not. For example, the fallacy is committed when a study contains the conclusion that TV advertising increases preference for sugar-based foods, but the reader later believes that the study concluded that TV advertising should be controlled. Previous research indicates that the commission of the fallacy was strong when memory for social research was tested. The present study showed that the commission of the fallacy was stronger when the research which was read involved children as subjects compared to adult subjects. Increased empathy for the subjects of a research article strengthens commission of the fallacy. 相似文献
75.
Trace mineral analysis of human hair was utilized to determine whether violent and nonviolent criminals could be differentiated on the basis of concentrations of levels of major and trace minerals. 40 violent and 40 nonviolent inmates from a prison population were selected for study. 27 mineral levels were analyzed and a discriminant function analysis correctly classified 86% of the violent and 78% of the nonviolent inmates. The results lent support to the hypothesis that significantly different levels of trace minerals could be found between the two groups. 相似文献
76.
Test of speech-sound discrimination are used by special educators, reading specialists and speech-language pathologists in assessing children's ability to differentiate between speech sounds occurring in standard English. Such tests are important in determining if speech-sound articulation errors are caused by difficulty in making such differentiations. However, during the past 10 years, these tests have been criticized on the basis of their reliability and validity. The purpose of this study was to examine the use of two alternative methods of assessing speech-sound discrimination with a school-aged population to determine if they elicited responses in a similar manner. 相似文献
77.
H-reflex amplitudes were studied during the acquisition of a motor skill involving coordinated isometric plantarflexion at the ankle joints as subjects learned to trace a triangular pattern on an oscilloscope screen by controlling plantarflexion torque applied against load cells. Torque feedback was presented on a subject oscilloscope with the right foot controlling a vertical cursor and the left foot a horizontal cursor. Eleven subjects reached criterion performance. H-reflexes were recorded from the right soleus and timed to the initiation of soleus muscle activity to plantarflex the foot. Average time to complete one trial decreased from 9 sec. in the prelearning block to 4 sec. postlearning. No single learning strategy was evident as subjects varied widely in their levels of H-reflex amplitude, but during postlearning, H-reflex amplitude became consistent within subjects as tracing performance became faster and accurate. 相似文献
78.
79.
A model of perceptual classification in children and adults 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
L B Smith 《Psychological review》1989,96(1):125-144
The developmental trend from overall-similarity to dimensional-identity classifications is explained by a quantitative model. I begin with the assumption that objects are represented in terms of constituent dimensions and that the representation of objects changes little with development. Given this assumption, the model has three major parts. First, the similarity between objects is a function of the combination of the constituent dimensional differences. I propose developmental change in the likelihood that dimensions are differentially weighted in the calculation of similarity. Second, the perceived similarities between objects are valued for the purpose of constructing classifications. I propose that similarities are valued more dichotomously with age, such that identity becomes increasingly special. Third, the valued similarities are used to choose the best classification of those possible. The model provides good qualitative fits to the extant data. Three experiments examining classifications in 2- to 8-year-olds and in adults support specific new claims of the model. The data and the model provide new insights about development, classification, and similarity. 相似文献
80.
A neuroscience model of stuttering 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
H.B. Nudelman K.E. Herbrich B.D. Hoyt D.B. Rosenfield 《Journal of Fluency Disorders》1989,14(6):399-427
Using motor control systems analysis and a reductionist approach, we provide a unified model of stuttering. This model views stuttering as a momentary instability in a complex multiloop control system. The model predicts the temporal conditions under which this instability will occur. Furthermore, these temporal conditions account for the efficacy of fluency-evoking maneuvers, therapy, and the variability of speech output in stutterers. 相似文献