全文获取类型
收费全文 | 403篇 |
免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
409篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 9篇 |
2018年 | 11篇 |
2017年 | 24篇 |
2016年 | 16篇 |
2015年 | 6篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 46篇 |
2012年 | 30篇 |
2011年 | 17篇 |
2010年 | 11篇 |
2009年 | 12篇 |
2008年 | 20篇 |
2007年 | 17篇 |
2006年 | 19篇 |
2005年 | 7篇 |
2004年 | 10篇 |
2003年 | 17篇 |
2002年 | 14篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1965年 | 1篇 |
1964年 | 1篇 |
1948年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有409条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
Prior investigators have asserted that certain group characteristics cause group members to disregard outside information and that this behavior leads to diminished performance. We demonstrate that the very process of making a judgment collaboratively rather than individually also contributes to such myopic underweighting of external viewpoints. Dyad members exposed to numerical judgments made by peers gave significantly less weight to those judgments than did individuals working alone. This difference in willingness to use peer input was mediated by the greater confidence that the dyad members reported in the accuracy of their own estimates. Furthermore, dyads were no better at judging the relative accuracy of their own estimates and the advisor's estimates than individuals were. Our analyses demonstrate that, relative to individuals, dyads suffered an accuracy cost. Specifically, if dyad members had given as much weight to peer input as individuals working alone did, then their revised estimates would have been significantly more accurate. 相似文献
132.
Piper BJ Li V Eiwaz MA Kobel YV Benice TS Chu AM Olsen RH Rice DZ Gray HM Mueller ST Raber J 《Behavior research methods》2012,44(1):110-123
The measurement of executive function has a long history in clinical and experimental neuropsychology. The goal of the present report was to determine the profile
of behavior across the lifespan on four computerized measures of executive function contained in the recently developed Psychology
Experiment Building Language (PEBL) test battery and evaluate whether this pattern is comparable to data previously obtained with the non-PEBL versions of these tests. Participants
(N = 1,223; ages, 5–89 years) completed the PEBL Trail Making Test (pTMT), the Wisconsin Card Sort Test (pWCST; Berg, Journal of General Psychology, 39, 15–22, 1948; Grant & Berg, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 404–411, 1948), the Tower of London (pToL), or a time estimation task (Time-Wall). Age-related effects were found over all four tests,
especially as age increased from young childhood through adulthood. For several tests and measures (including pToL and pTMT),
age-related slowing was found as age increased in adulthood. Together, these findings indicate that the PEBL tests provide
valid and versatile new research tools for measuring executive functions. 相似文献
133.
134.
Thomas T. Liu Gary H. Glover Bryon A. Mueller Douglas N. Greve Gregory G. Brown 《Psychometrika》2013,78(2):308-321
In functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), the blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) signal is often interpreted as a measure of neural activity. However, because the BOLD signal reflects the complex interplay of neural, vascular, and metabolic processes, such an interpretation is not always valid. There is growing evidence that changes in the baseline neurovascular state can result in significant modulations of the BOLD signal that are independent of changes in neural activity. This paper introduces some of the normalization and calibration methods that have been proposed for making the BOLD signal a more accurate reflection of underlying brain activity for human fMRI studies. 相似文献
135.
136.
Exline JJ Park CL Smyth JM Carey MP 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2011,101(1):129-148
A recent model of collective action distinguishes 2 distinct pathways: an emotional pathway whereby anger in response to injustice motivates action and an efficacy pathway where the belief that issues can be solved collectively increases the likelihood that group members take action (van Zomeren, Spears, Fischer, & Leach, 2004). Research supporting this model has, however, focused entirely on relatively normative actions such as participating in demonstrations. We argue that the relations between emotions, efficacy, and action differ for more extreme, nonnormative actions and propose (a) that nonnormative actions are often driven by a sense of low efficacy and (b) that contempt, which, unlike anger, entails psychological distancing and a lack of reconciliatory intentions, predicts nonnormative action. These ideas were tested in 3 survey studies examining student protests against tuition fees in Germany (N = 332), Indian Muslims' action support in relation to ingroup disadvantage (N = 156), and British Muslims' responses to British foreign policy (N = 466). Results were generally supportive of predictions and indicated that (a) anger was strongly related to normative action but overall unrelated or less strongly related to nonnormative action, (b) contempt was either unrelated or negatively related to normative action but significantly positively predicted nonnormative action, and (c) efficacy was positively related to normative action and negatively related to nonnormative action. The implications of these findings for understanding and dealing with extreme intergroup phenomena such as terrorism are discussed. 相似文献
137.
Cross Racial Identity Scale (CRIS) scores: stability and relationships with psychological adjustment
Worrell FC Mendoza-Denton R Telesford J Simmons C Martin JF 《Journal of personality assessment》2011,93(6):637-648
We examined the structural validity, internal consistency (alpha and omega), and test-retest reliability of scores on the Cross Racial Identity Scale (CRIS; Vandiver et al., 2000 ; Worrell, Vandiver, & Cross, 2004 ), as well as the relationship between CRIS scores and several variables related to psychological adjustment. Participants consisted of several groups of African American college students (34 ≤ n ≤ 340) attending a predominantly White university in a Western state. Confirmatory factor analyses indicated an acceptable fit of the data to the theoretical model, and alpha and omega coefficients indicate that CRIS scores have moderate to high internal consistency. CRIS scores also demonstrated stability over periods between 2 and 20 months in ranges that suggest long-term stability of racial attitudes. As predicted by the expanded nigrescence model (Cross & Vandiver, 2001 ), only self-hatred attitudes had consistent, meaningful relationships with psychological adjustment. 相似文献
138.
Axel Mueller 《Synthese》2011,182(3):449-473
Standard interpretations of Kant’s transcendental idealism take it as a commitment to the view that the objects of cognition
are structured or made by conditions imposed by the mind, and therefore to what Van Cleve calls “honest-to-God idealism”.
Against this view, many more recent investigations of Kant’s theory of representation and cognitive significance have been
able to show that Kant is committed to a certain form of Mental Content Externalism, and therefore to the realist view that
the objects involved in experience and empirical knowledge are mind-independent particulars. Some of these recent interpreters
have taken this result to demonstrate an internal incompatibility between Kant‘s transcendental idealism and his own model
of cognitive content and the environmental conditions of empirical knowledge. Against this suggestion, this article argues
that, while Kant’s theory of content is indeed best construed as externalist, an adequately adjusted form of transcendental
idealism is not only compatible with this externalism, but in fact supports it. More generally, the article develops the position
that mental content externalism cannot force the adoption of metaphysical realism. 相似文献
139.
140.
Nicolas Fay Andrew C Page Crystal Serfaty 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2010,46(4):689-692
People have an exaggerated sense of self-efficacy. This paper investigates the mechanism behind speakers’ tendency to overestimate their communication efficacy. Two competing mechanisms were tested: intrapersonal and interpersonal. Speakers communicated a series of ambiguous statements to a co-present or absent listener and then estimated their success. Speakers overestimated their ability to communicate their intended message (relative to listeners’ actual comprehension of their message) when a listener was co-present, but not when a listener was absent. Our findings support an interpersonal explanation of speakers’ perceived communication efficacy. 相似文献