全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1061篇 |
免费 | 45篇 |
专业分类
1106篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 19篇 |
2019年 | 23篇 |
2018年 | 23篇 |
2017年 | 18篇 |
2016年 | 39篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 21篇 |
2013年 | 108篇 |
2012年 | 27篇 |
2011年 | 34篇 |
2010年 | 27篇 |
2009年 | 17篇 |
2008年 | 31篇 |
2007年 | 30篇 |
2006年 | 29篇 |
2005年 | 25篇 |
2004年 | 25篇 |
2003年 | 24篇 |
2002年 | 23篇 |
2001年 | 27篇 |
2000年 | 22篇 |
1999年 | 23篇 |
1998年 | 21篇 |
1997年 | 13篇 |
1996年 | 13篇 |
1995年 | 15篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 12篇 |
1992年 | 19篇 |
1991年 | 26篇 |
1990年 | 17篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 20篇 |
1987年 | 15篇 |
1986年 | 13篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 19篇 |
1983年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 17篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 14篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 20篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
1972年 | 13篇 |
1967年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有1106条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
61.
The relative influence of perceptual and semantic features on naming performance was investigated with reference to the neurobehavioral profiles displayed by patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD). Forty-one patients were classified as manifesting a verbal, visual, or global subtype based upon their pattern of neuropsychological functioning. Perceptual characteristics of to-be-named pictures were varied by manipulating the amount of line detail, whereas semantic qualities were varied by altering word frequency norms. All AD subtypes were less accurate than normal elderly controls in naming low frequency pictures. Patients and controls took longer to name low frequency and high complexity pictures, and this effect was comparable across the AD groups. Patients with predominantly visual deficits were significantly slower in naming than controls, and those with verbal impairments made a higher proportion of semantic naming errors when compared to patients displaying visual or severe global impairments. These results suggest that deficits in semantic processing contribute to naming dysfunction in AD, and they highlight the importance of examining dissociations among neurobehavioral subtypes. 相似文献
62.
It has become increasingly recognized that cognitive therapy (CT) is an effective treatment for the positive symptoms of schizophrenia yet there are few cognitive therapists in North America who are specialized to work with this patient population. There is a need for further dissemination of CT for schizophrenia in order to increase its availability. A first step in dissemination is to become familiar with the cognitive theory and therapy model, as applied to schizophrenia. The purpose of the present paper is to demonstrate the applicability of CT for psychosis, using the example of command hallucinations. First, we summarize the cognitive theory of command hallucinations and present the stages of CT, using the example of “Joe,” a 24-year-old male with schizophrenia. The paper concludes with practical suggestions of how to enhance the effectiveness of CT for command hallucinations. 相似文献
63.
A multidisciplinary group of genetic and other professionals discuss a complex case of familial conflict over access to genetic information. One twin wishes to know the results of her deceased mother's tests for BRCA1 mutations; a second twin objects to researchers making this information available. As the case discussion unfolds, the apparent facts of this case are called into question and the lines of ethical certainty become less clear. 相似文献
64.
In this study we explored the views of Mental Health professionals and general practitioners (GPs) regarding spiritual care and the effect of personal and cultural background on their views. Data were collected through anonymous questionnaires posted to hospital nursing and medical staff at Hollins Park Hospital, Warrington, UK, and to all GPs within the hospital catchments area. Forty-five percent of GPs, 33% of psychiatrists, and 76% of nursing staff (p < 0.05) felt strongly that human beings are made up of spirit as well as body. More nurses felt that spiritual care is equally as important as other forms of care (52%) compared with psychiatrists (33%) and GPs (29%). A higher percentage of nursing than medical staff had previous training in this area and were more likely than medical staff to consider themselves appropriate to give spiritual advice. A total of 20% of GPs and 33% of psychiatric staff stated the need for training in this area and GPs especially felt they lacked time. Professionals’ views are influenced by cultural and religious backgrounds, with significantly more non-UK born respondents feeling strongly that human beings all have a spiritual component. Many doctors do not consider spiritual care has a role for them to be involved in and many feel too pressured in daily life to take this on. Nurses are more inclined to take a holistic approach to care in this respect. Some people with mental health problems have spiritual care needs and we should be aware of this as an important facet to therapy. 相似文献
65.
66.
John Green 《欧洲心理治疗、咨询与健康杂志》2013,15(1):150-152
Ann Scott (1996) Real Events Revisited: Fantasy, Memory and Psychoanalysis, London: Virago, $12.99 Caroline Malone, Linda Farthing and Lorraine Marce (eds) (1996) The Memory Bird: Survivors of Sexual Abuse, London: Virago, $8.99 相似文献
67.
Prudence Green 《欧洲心理治疗、咨询与健康杂志》2013,15(1):152-154
Alex Howard (1996) Challenges to Counselling and Psychotherapy, London: Macmillan, $11.99 相似文献
68.
James B. Worthen Gabriella Garcia-Rivas Charles R. Green Rebecca A. Vidos 《The Journal of general psychology》2013,140(2):117-144
The authors examined the role of cognitive-resource allocation in obtaining the often reported memory advantage for bizarre relative to common information by using the secondary-task method in 4 experiments. In Experiments 1 and 2, they investigated the relationship between differences in cognitive resource allocation during comprehension and in recall for common and bizarre information. In Experiments 3 and 4, they investigated the same relationship but measured cognitive-resource allocation during imaging rather than during comprehension. Although bizarre items required more time to comprehend and to image than did common items, the differences were not reliable predictors of the recall differences between item types. Furthermore, analyses of response latencies to a secondary task provided no compelling evidence in support of a cognitive-resource-allocation explanation of bizarreness effects. Implications for the development of a comprehensive model of the influence of bizarreness on memory are discussed. 相似文献
69.
Green RM 《Kennedy Institute of Ethics journal》2005,15(1):101-106
H.G. Wells warned, in 1895, not to allow economic injustices to become so acute that they ultimately transform human biology. Wells's warning is all the more pertinent today as society contemplates the use of biotechnologies to manipulate or "enhance" the human genome. 相似文献
70.
Broman-Fulks JJ Green BA Olatunji BO Berman ME Arnau RC Deacon BJ Sawchuk CN 《Assessment》2008,15(2):188-203
Anxiety sensitivity has been implicated as a risk factor for the development and maintenance of panic and other anxiety disorders. Although researchers have generally assumed that anxiety sensitivity is a dimensional, rather than categorical, variable, recent taxometric research has raised questions concerning the accuracy of this assumption. The present study examined the latent structure of anxiety sensitivity by applying four taxometric procedures (MAXEIG, MAXCOV, MAMBAC, and L-Mode) to data collected from two large nonclinical samples (n = 1,025 and n = 744) using two distinct measures of anxiety sensitivity (Anxiety Sensitivity Profile and Anxiety Sensitivity Index-Revised). In contrast to previous taxometric analyses of anxiety sensitivity, results of the present research provided convergent evidence for a latent anxiety sensitivity dimension. Several potential explanations for the discrepancy between these findings and those of previous research are discussed, as well as the implications of these findings for the conceptualization and measurement of anxiety sensitivity. 相似文献