全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1061篇 |
免费 | 45篇 |
专业分类
1106篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 19篇 |
2019年 | 23篇 |
2018年 | 23篇 |
2017年 | 18篇 |
2016年 | 39篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 21篇 |
2013年 | 108篇 |
2012年 | 27篇 |
2011年 | 34篇 |
2010年 | 27篇 |
2009年 | 17篇 |
2008年 | 31篇 |
2007年 | 30篇 |
2006年 | 29篇 |
2005年 | 25篇 |
2004年 | 25篇 |
2003年 | 24篇 |
2002年 | 23篇 |
2001年 | 27篇 |
2000年 | 22篇 |
1999年 | 23篇 |
1998年 | 21篇 |
1997年 | 13篇 |
1996年 | 13篇 |
1995年 | 15篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 12篇 |
1992年 | 19篇 |
1991年 | 26篇 |
1990年 | 17篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 20篇 |
1987年 | 15篇 |
1986年 | 13篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 19篇 |
1983年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 17篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 14篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 20篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
1972年 | 13篇 |
1967年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有1106条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
211.
This paper examines a representative case of histrionic (the term hysterical is used interchangably) personality from a psychological and theological perspective. A working hypothesis is presented, based on Genesis 1–3, that is both clinical and theological. It is hypothesized that individuals who manifest histrionic features relate to each other as Strong Man/Dependent Woman and/or Strong Woman/Passive Man. Both ways of relating are usually present in the same relationship. In relating to one another in these ways, they are searching for a human god or goddess. In turn, they think and act as if they too were a god or goddess. They have not discovered another way of relating; that is, as male and female created in the image of God. A clinical case study is presented and the course of treatment described. Since the histrionic personality is multidimensional, so must be the treatment. 相似文献
212.
Assume that each presentation of a signal produces two independent random variable representations and that the ratio of responses on successive trials of a magnitude estimation experiment are proportional to the ratio of a representation from the present trial, which representation is then lost, to the remaining one from the previous trial. The mean response to a particular signal depends on the mean of the representation used, but in general exhibits drift over trials and sequential effects due to the preceeding trial; the mean response ratio does not exhibit drift, but it has a simple form only when there are no sequential effects; however, a modified mean ratio function has a simple form. A model suggested by D. V. Cross is a special case of this one. Simple timing and counting models for the representations fail to exhibit sequential effects, contrary to considerable data. However, data of the authors have suggested a version of the timing model in which the sample size of the representation varies by an order of magnitude depending on how close the signal is to the preceding one; this hypothesis accounts for the observed sequential effects and other aspects of the data. 相似文献
213.
214.
Willard P. Green 《Theoretical medicine and bioethics》1988,9(1):33-44
Although we normally have no difficulty with holding individuals accountable for the effects of their actions, we are still confused about holding a health care team accountable. I argue that we can hold teams accountable in the same way that we hold individuals accountable. In constructing this argument, I first examine the nature of a “team,” then look at the consequences of team decision and action, in particular, the problem of synergistic decisionmaking. Finally I relate this philosophical discussion to patient care decisions by teams. 相似文献
215.
Distance perception of depicted objects was examined as a function of photographic area of view. Subjects viewed slides of natural outdoor scenes and directly estimated the distances to specified objects. Area of view was manipulated by means of photographing each scene with lenses of five different focal lengths: 135, 80, 48, 28, and 17 mm. Distance perception along the pictorial depth plane was systematically transformed through changing the photographic area of view: the shorter the focal length of the camera lens, the wider the area of view, and the greater the perceived distance. Linear functions for each subject's distance judgments revealed a very high goodness of fit. Both the y-intercepts and the slopes increased as focal length decreased. The increasing y-intercepts suggest that viewers place themselves farther away from the depicted scene as focal length decreases, compensating for the advancing proximal edge. The increasing slopes suggest that distance information throughout the pictorial depth plane appears to change with focal length. The subjects also made direct judgments of foreground truncation, revealing that foreground truncation decreased as focal length decreased, but that this decrease did not account for the considerable expansion in distance perception. 相似文献
216.
217.
R.Duncan Luce John C Baird David M Green Albert F Smith 《Journal of mathematical psychology》1980,22(2):121-148
One class of models assumes that presentation of a signal results in an internal representation as a random variable. Depending on whether the signal is close to or far from the preceding signal, the variance of the representation is smaller or larger. Responses are determined largely by this random variable; however, when the signal is close to the preceding one, the response is generated by modifying the representation multiplicatively by some function of the ratio of the previous response to its representation. Power and linear functions are explored. The form of the random variable is assumed to be that arising from either the timing or the counting model operating on a Poisson process. Detailed analyses are carried out successfully only for the timing model with neural sample sizes independent of intensity; however, the data require the sample to increase with intensity. The linear response function coupled with the constant sample size counting model appears somewhat viable, but detailed calculations are very difficult to carry out. The second class of models postulates a power function relation between magnitude estimates and signals intensity for which the exponent is a Gaussian distributed random variable and the unit is the product of two log normal random variables. Again we assume an attention band such that succesive stimuli that are widely separated in intensity lead to independent samples of the random variables while a variety of assumptions is explored for successive stimuli that are near each other in intensity. Although they each give rise to the qualitative features of the data, estimates of parameters are sufficiently inconsistent that we are led to reject all of the submodels studied. 相似文献
218.
J C Baird D M Green R D Luce 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1980,6(2):277-289
Individual subjects' performance was examined for cross-modality matching (CMM) of loudness to visual area, as well as for magnitude estimation (ME) of the component continua. Average exponents of power functions relating response magnitude to stimulus intensity were .73 for area, .20 for loudness, and 2.44 for CMM. Predictions of the CMM exponent based on ME were higher than the empirical values, whereas more accurate predictions were made from magnitude production exponents obtained in a previous study. Sequential dependencies were assessed by comparing the response on trial n to the response on trial n--1. The coefficient of variation of the response ratio Rn/Rn-1 was systematically related to the stimulus ratio Sn/Sn-1 for both area and loudness. The coefficient was lowest for ratios near 1 and increased for larger or smaller values. For CMM, the coefficient of variation appeared to be independent of stimulus ratios. The correlation between log Rn and log Rn-1 was also related to Sn/Sn-1 for both ME and CMM. The correlation was highest when Sn/Sn-1 was 1 and dropped to 0 with increasing stimulus separation, but CMM yielded a shallower function than ME. 相似文献
219.
220.